• Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

How to Write a Great Hypothesis

Hypothesis Definition, Format, Examples, and Tips

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

research hypothesis example psychology

Amy Morin, LCSW, is a psychotherapist and international bestselling author. Her books, including "13 Things Mentally Strong People Don't Do," have been translated into more than 40 languages. Her TEDx talk,  "The Secret of Becoming Mentally Strong," is one of the most viewed talks of all time.

research hypothesis example psychology

Verywell / Alex Dos Diaz

  • The Scientific Method

Hypothesis Format

Falsifiability of a hypothesis.

  • Operationalization

Hypothesis Types

Hypotheses examples.

  • Collecting Data

A hypothesis is a tentative statement about the relationship between two or more variables. It is a specific, testable prediction about what you expect to happen in a study. It is a preliminary answer to your question that helps guide the research process.

Consider a study designed to examine the relationship between sleep deprivation and test performance. The hypothesis might be: "This study is designed to assess the hypothesis that sleep-deprived people will perform worse on a test than individuals who are not sleep-deprived."

At a Glance

A hypothesis is crucial to scientific research because it offers a clear direction for what the researchers are looking to find. This allows them to design experiments to test their predictions and add to our scientific knowledge about the world. This article explores how a hypothesis is used in psychology research, how to write a good hypothesis, and the different types of hypotheses you might use.

The Hypothesis in the Scientific Method

In the scientific method , whether it involves research in psychology, biology, or some other area, a hypothesis represents what the researchers think will happen in an experiment. The scientific method involves the following steps:

  • Forming a question
  • Performing background research
  • Creating a hypothesis
  • Designing an experiment
  • Collecting data
  • Analyzing the results
  • Drawing conclusions
  • Communicating the results

The hypothesis is a prediction, but it involves more than a guess. Most of the time, the hypothesis begins with a question which is then explored through background research. At this point, researchers then begin to develop a testable hypothesis.

Unless you are creating an exploratory study, your hypothesis should always explain what you  expect  to happen.

In a study exploring the effects of a particular drug, the hypothesis might be that researchers expect the drug to have some type of effect on the symptoms of a specific illness. In psychology, the hypothesis might focus on how a certain aspect of the environment might influence a particular behavior.

Remember, a hypothesis does not have to be correct. While the hypothesis predicts what the researchers expect to see, the goal of the research is to determine whether this guess is right or wrong. When conducting an experiment, researchers might explore numerous factors to determine which ones might contribute to the ultimate outcome.

In many cases, researchers may find that the results of an experiment  do not  support the original hypothesis. When writing up these results, the researchers might suggest other options that should be explored in future studies.

In many cases, researchers might draw a hypothesis from a specific theory or build on previous research. For example, prior research has shown that stress can impact the immune system. So a researcher might hypothesize: "People with high-stress levels will be more likely to contract a common cold after being exposed to the virus than people who have low-stress levels."

In other instances, researchers might look at commonly held beliefs or folk wisdom. "Birds of a feather flock together" is one example of folk adage that a psychologist might try to investigate. The researcher might pose a specific hypothesis that "People tend to select romantic partners who are similar to them in interests and educational level."

Elements of a Good Hypothesis

So how do you write a good hypothesis? When trying to come up with a hypothesis for your research or experiments, ask yourself the following questions:

  • Is your hypothesis based on your research on a topic?
  • Can your hypothesis be tested?
  • Does your hypothesis include independent and dependent variables?

Before you come up with a specific hypothesis, spend some time doing background research. Once you have completed a literature review, start thinking about potential questions you still have. Pay attention to the discussion section in the  journal articles you read . Many authors will suggest questions that still need to be explored.

How to Formulate a Good Hypothesis

To form a hypothesis, you should take these steps:

  • Collect as many observations about a topic or problem as you can.
  • Evaluate these observations and look for possible causes of the problem.
  • Create a list of possible explanations that you might want to explore.
  • After you have developed some possible hypotheses, think of ways that you could confirm or disprove each hypothesis through experimentation. This is known as falsifiability.

In the scientific method ,  falsifiability is an important part of any valid hypothesis. In order to test a claim scientifically, it must be possible that the claim could be proven false.

Students sometimes confuse the idea of falsifiability with the idea that it means that something is false, which is not the case. What falsifiability means is that  if  something was false, then it is possible to demonstrate that it is false.

One of the hallmarks of pseudoscience is that it makes claims that cannot be refuted or proven false.

The Importance of Operational Definitions

A variable is a factor or element that can be changed and manipulated in ways that are observable and measurable. However, the researcher must also define how the variable will be manipulated and measured in the study.

Operational definitions are specific definitions for all relevant factors in a study. This process helps make vague or ambiguous concepts detailed and measurable.

For example, a researcher might operationally define the variable " test anxiety " as the results of a self-report measure of anxiety experienced during an exam. A "study habits" variable might be defined by the amount of studying that actually occurs as measured by time.

These precise descriptions are important because many things can be measured in various ways. Clearly defining these variables and how they are measured helps ensure that other researchers can replicate your results.

Replicability

One of the basic principles of any type of scientific research is that the results must be replicable.

Replication means repeating an experiment in the same way to produce the same results. By clearly detailing the specifics of how the variables were measured and manipulated, other researchers can better understand the results and repeat the study if needed.

Some variables are more difficult than others to define. For example, how would you operationally define a variable such as aggression ? For obvious ethical reasons, researchers cannot create a situation in which a person behaves aggressively toward others.

To measure this variable, the researcher must devise a measurement that assesses aggressive behavior without harming others. The researcher might utilize a simulated task to measure aggressiveness in this situation.

Hypothesis Checklist

  • Does your hypothesis focus on something that you can actually test?
  • Does your hypothesis include both an independent and dependent variable?
  • Can you manipulate the variables?
  • Can your hypothesis be tested without violating ethical standards?

The hypothesis you use will depend on what you are investigating and hoping to find. Some of the main types of hypotheses that you might use include:

  • Simple hypothesis : This type of hypothesis suggests there is a relationship between one independent variable and one dependent variable.
  • Complex hypothesis : This type suggests a relationship between three or more variables, such as two independent and dependent variables.
  • Null hypothesis : This hypothesis suggests no relationship exists between two or more variables.
  • Alternative hypothesis : This hypothesis states the opposite of the null hypothesis.
  • Statistical hypothesis : This hypothesis uses statistical analysis to evaluate a representative population sample and then generalizes the findings to the larger group.
  • Logical hypothesis : This hypothesis assumes a relationship between variables without collecting data or evidence.

A hypothesis often follows a basic format of "If {this happens} then {this will happen}." One way to structure your hypothesis is to describe what will happen to the  dependent variable  if you change the  independent variable .

The basic format might be: "If {these changes are made to a certain independent variable}, then we will observe {a change in a specific dependent variable}."

A few examples of simple hypotheses:

  • "Students who eat breakfast will perform better on a math exam than students who do not eat breakfast."
  • "Students who experience test anxiety before an English exam will get lower scores than students who do not experience test anxiety."​
  • "Motorists who talk on the phone while driving will be more likely to make errors on a driving course than those who do not talk on the phone."
  • "Children who receive a new reading intervention will have higher reading scores than students who do not receive the intervention."

Examples of a complex hypothesis include:

  • "People with high-sugar diets and sedentary activity levels are more likely to develop depression."
  • "Younger people who are regularly exposed to green, outdoor areas have better subjective well-being than older adults who have limited exposure to green spaces."

Examples of a null hypothesis include:

  • "There is no difference in anxiety levels between people who take St. John's wort supplements and those who do not."
  • "There is no difference in scores on a memory recall task between children and adults."
  • "There is no difference in aggression levels between children who play first-person shooter games and those who do not."

Examples of an alternative hypothesis:

  • "People who take St. John's wort supplements will have less anxiety than those who do not."
  • "Adults will perform better on a memory task than children."
  • "Children who play first-person shooter games will show higher levels of aggression than children who do not." 

Collecting Data on Your Hypothesis

Once a researcher has formed a testable hypothesis, the next step is to select a research design and start collecting data. The research method depends largely on exactly what they are studying. There are two basic types of research methods: descriptive research and experimental research.

Descriptive Research Methods

Descriptive research such as  case studies ,  naturalistic observations , and surveys are often used when  conducting an experiment is difficult or impossible. These methods are best used to describe different aspects of a behavior or psychological phenomenon.

Once a researcher has collected data using descriptive methods, a  correlational study  can examine how the variables are related. This research method might be used to investigate a hypothesis that is difficult to test experimentally.

Experimental Research Methods

Experimental methods  are used to demonstrate causal relationships between variables. In an experiment, the researcher systematically manipulates a variable of interest (known as the independent variable) and measures the effect on another variable (known as the dependent variable).

Unlike correlational studies, which can only be used to determine if there is a relationship between two variables, experimental methods can be used to determine the actual nature of the relationship—whether changes in one variable actually  cause  another to change.

The hypothesis is a critical part of any scientific exploration. It represents what researchers expect to find in a study or experiment. In situations where the hypothesis is unsupported by the research, the research still has value. Such research helps us better understand how different aspects of the natural world relate to one another. It also helps us develop new hypotheses that can then be tested in the future.

Thompson WH, Skau S. On the scope of scientific hypotheses .  R Soc Open Sci . 2023;10(8):230607. doi:10.1098/rsos.230607

Taran S, Adhikari NKJ, Fan E. Falsifiability in medicine: what clinicians can learn from Karl Popper [published correction appears in Intensive Care Med. 2021 Jun 17;:].  Intensive Care Med . 2021;47(9):1054-1056. doi:10.1007/s00134-021-06432-z

Eyler AA. Research Methods for Public Health . 1st ed. Springer Publishing Company; 2020. doi:10.1891/9780826182067.0004

Nosek BA, Errington TM. What is replication ?  PLoS Biol . 2020;18(3):e3000691. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000691

Aggarwal R, Ranganathan P. Study designs: Part 2 - Descriptive studies .  Perspect Clin Res . 2019;10(1):34-36. doi:10.4103/picr.PICR_154_18

Nevid J. Psychology: Concepts and Applications. Wadworth, 2013.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

6 Hypothesis Examples in Psychology

The hypothesis is one of the most important steps of psychological research. Hypothesis refers to an assumption or the temporary statement made by the researcher before the execution of the experiment, regarding the possible outcome of that experiment. A hypothesis can be tested through various scientific and statistical tools. It is a logical guess based on previous knowledge and investigations related to the problem under investigation. In this article, we’ll learn about the significance of the hypothesis, the sources of the hypothesis, and the various examples of the hypothesis.

Sources of Hypothesis

The formulation of a good hypothesis is not an easy task. One needs to take care of the various crucial steps to get an accurate hypothesis. The hypothesis formulation demands both the creativity of the researcher and his/her years of experience. The researcher needs to use critical thinking to avoid committing any errors such as choosing the wrong hypothesis. Although the hypothesis is considered the first step before further investigations such as data collection for the experiment, the hypothesis formulation also requires some amount of data collection. The data collection for the hypothesis formulation refers to the review of literature related to the concerned topic, and understanding of the previous research on the related topic. Following are some of the main sources of the hypothesis that may help the researcher to formulate a good hypothesis.

  • Reviewing the similar studies and literature related to a similar problem.
  • Examining the available data concerned with the problem.
  • Discussing the problem with the colleagues, or the professional researchers about the problem under investigation.
  • Thorough research and investigation by conducting field interviews or surveys on the people that are directly concerned with the problem under investigation.
  • Sometimes ‘institution’ of the well known and experienced researcher is also considered as a good source of the hypothesis formulation.

Real Life Hypothesis Examples

1. null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis examples.

Every research problem-solving procedure begins with the formulation of the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis. The alternative hypothesis assumes the existence of the relationship between the variables under study, while the null hypothesis denies the relationship between the variables under study. Following are examples of the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis based on the research problem.

Research Problem: What is the benefit of eating an apple daily on your health?

Alternative Hypothesis: Eating an apple daily reduces the chances of visiting the doctor.

Null Hypothesis : Eating an apple daily does not impact the frequency of visiting the doctor.

Research Problem: What is the impact of spending a lot of time on mobiles on the attention span of teenagers.

Alternative Problem: Spending time on the mobiles and attention span have a negative correlation.

Null Hypothesis: There does not exist any correlation between the use of mobile by teenagers on their attention span.

Research Problem: What is the impact of providing flexible working hours to the employees on the job satisfaction level.

Alternative Hypothesis : Employees who get the option of flexible working hours have better job satisfaction than the employees who don’t get the option of flexible working hours.

Null Hypothesis: There is no association between providing flexible working hours and job satisfaction.

2. Simple Hypothesis Examples

The hypothesis that includes only one independent variable (predictor variable) and one dependent variable (outcome variable) is termed the simple hypothesis. For example, the children are more likely to get clinical depression if their parents had also suffered from the clinical depression. Here, the independent variable is the parents suffering from clinical depression and the dependent or the outcome variable is the clinical depression observed in their child/children. Other examples of the simple hypothesis are given below,

  • If the management provides the official snack breaks to the employees, the employees are less likely to take the off-site breaks. Here, providing snack breaks is the independent variable and the employees are less likely to take the off-site break is the dependent variable.

3. Complex Hypothesis Examples

If the hypothesis includes more than one independent (predictor variable) or more than one dependent variable (outcome variable) it is known as the complex hypothesis. For example, clinical depression in children is associated with a family clinical depression history and a stressful and hectic lifestyle. In this case, there are two independent variables, i.e., family history of clinical depression and hectic and stressful lifestyle, and one dependent variable, i.e., clinical depression. Following are some more examples of the complex hypothesis,

4. Logical Hypothesis Examples

If there are not many pieces of evidence and studies related to the concerned problem, then the researcher can take the help of the general logic to formulate the hypothesis. The logical hypothesis is proved true through various logic. For example, if the researcher wants to prove that the animal needs water for its survival, then this can be logically verified through the logic that ‘living beings can not survive without the water.’ Following are some more examples of logical hypotheses,

  • Tia is not good at maths, hence she will not choose the accounting sector as her career.
  • If there is a correlation between skin cancer and ultraviolet rays, then the people who are more exposed to the ultraviolet rays are more prone to skin cancer.
  • The beings belonging to the different planets can not breathe in the earth’s atmosphere.
  • The creatures living in the sea use anaerobic respiration as those living outside the sea use aerobic respiration.

5. Empirical Hypothesis Examples

The empirical hypothesis comes into existence when the statement is being tested by conducting various experiments. This hypothesis is not just an idea or notion, instead, it refers to the statement that undergoes various trials and errors, and various extraneous variables can impact the result. The trials and errors provide a set of results that can be testable over time. Following are the examples of the empirical hypothesis,

  • The hungry cat will quickly reach the endpoint through the maze, if food is placed at the endpoint then the cat is not hungry.
  • The people who consume vitamin c have more glowing skin than the people who consume vitamin E.
  • Hair growth is faster after the consumption of Vitamin E than vitamin K.
  • Plants will grow faster with fertilizer X than with fertilizer Y.

6. Statistical Hypothesis Examples

The statements that can be proven true by using the various statistical tools are considered the statistical hypothesis. The researcher uses statistical data about an area or the group in the analysis of the statistical hypothesis. For example, if you study the IQ level of the women belonging to nation X, it would be practically impossible to measure the IQ level of each woman belonging to nation X. Here, statistical methods come to the rescue. The researcher can choose the sample population, i.e., women belonging to the different states or provinces of the nation X, and conduct the statistical tests on this sample population to get the average IQ of the women belonging to the nation X. Following are the examples of the statistical hypothesis.

  • 30 per cent of the women belonging to the nation X are working.
  • 50 per cent of the people living in the savannah are above the age of 70 years.
  • 45 per cent of the poor people in the United States are uneducated.

Significance of Hypothesis

A hypothesis is very crucial in experimental research as it aims to predict any particular outcome of the experiment. Hypothesis plays an important role in guiding the researchers to focus on the concerned area of research only. However, the hypothesis is not required by all researchers. The type of research that seeks for finding facts, i.e., historical research, does not need the formulation of the hypothesis. In the historical research, the researchers look for the pieces of evidence related to the human life, the history of a particular area, or the occurrence of any event, this means that the researcher does not have a strong basis to make an assumption in these types of researches, hence hypothesis is not needed in this case. As stated by Hillway (1964)

When fact-finding alone is the aim of the study, a hypothesis is not required.”

The hypothesis may not be an important part of the descriptive or historical studies, but it is a crucial part for the experimental researchers. Following are some of the points that show the importance of formulating a hypothesis before conducting the experiment.

  • Hypothesis provides a tentative statement about the outcome of the experiment that can be validated and tested. It helps the researcher to directly focus on the problem under investigation by collecting the relevant data according to the variables mentioned in the hypothesis.
  • Hypothesis facilitates a direction to the experimental research. It helps the researcher in analysing what is relevant for the study and what’s not. It prevents the researcher’s time as he does not need to waste time on reviewing the irrelevant research and literature, and also prevents the researcher from collecting the irrelevant data.
  • Hypothesis helps the researcher in choosing the appropriate sample, statistical tests to conduct, variables to be studied and the research methodology. The hypothesis also helps the study from being generalised as it focuses on the limited and exact problem under investigation.
  • Hypothesis act as a framework for deducing the outcomes of the experiment. The researcher can easily test the different hypotheses for understanding the interaction among the various variables involved in the study. On this basis of the results obtained from the testing of various hypotheses, the researcher can formulate the final meaningful report.

Related Posts

Spearman’s Two-factor Theory of Intelligence Explained

Spearman’s Two-factor Theory of Intelligence Explained

Convergent Thinking Examples

Convergent Thinking Examples

Correlation Examples in Real Life

Correlation Examples in Real Life

Overconfidence Bias Examples

Overconfidence Bias Examples

Epiphany Examples in Real Life

Epiphany Examples in Real Life

Psychology and Other Disciplines

Psychology and Other Disciplines

Add comment cancel reply.

Find Study Materials for

  • Explanations
  • Business Studies
  • Combined Science
  • Computer Science
  • Engineering
  • English Literature
  • Environmental Science
  • Human Geography
  • Macroeconomics
  • Microeconomics
  • Social Studies
  • Browse all subjects
  • Read our Magazine

Create Study Materials

  • Flashcards Create and find the best flashcards.
  • Notes Create notes faster than ever before.
  • Study Sets Everything you need for your studies in one place.
  • Study Plans Stop procrastinating with our smart planner features.
  • Aims and Hypotheses

There is no research without a proper aim and hypotheses – aims and hypotheses in research are the supporting frameworks on a path to new scientific discoveries. To better understand their importance, let us first analyse the difference between aims and hypotheses in psychology , examine their purpose, and give some examples.

Aims and Hypotheses

Create learning materials about Aims and Hypotheses with our free learning app!

  • Instand access to millions of learning materials
  • Flashcards, notes, mock-exams and more
  • Everything you need to ace your exams
  • Approaches in Psychology
  • Basic Psychology
  • Biological Bases of Behavior
  • Biopsychology
  • Careers in Psychology
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognition and Development
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Data Handling and Analysis
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Eating Behaviour
  • Emotion and Motivation
  • Famous Psychologists
  • Forensic Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • Individual Differences Psychology
  • Issues and Debates in Psychology
  • Personality in Psychology
  • Psychological Treatment
  • Relationships
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Causation in Psychology
  • Coding Frame Psychology
  • Correlational Studies
  • Cross Cultural Research
  • Cross Sectional Research
  • Ethical Issues and Ways of Dealing with Them
  • Experimental Designs
  • Features of Science
  • Field Experiment
  • Independent Group Design
  • Lab Experiment
  • Longitudinal Research
  • Matched Pairs Design
  • Meta Analysis
  • Natural Experiment
  • Observational Design
  • Online Research
  • Paradigms and Falsifiability
  • Peer Review and Economic Applications of Research
  • Pilot Studies and the Aims of Piloting
  • Quality Criteria
  • Questionnaire Construction
  • Repeated Measures Design
  • Research Methods
  • Sampling Frames
  • Sampling Psychology
  • Scientific Processes
  • Scientific Report
  • Scientific Research
  • Self-Report Design
  • Self-Report Techniques
  • Semantic Differential Rating Scale
  • Snowball Sampling
  • Schizophrenia
  • Scientific Foundations of Psychology
  • Scientific Investigation
  • Sensation and Perception
  • Social Context of Behaviour
  • Social Psychology

First, we will define the aims and hypotheses and learn the difference between aims and hypotheses in psychology .

Then, we will look at different types of hypotheses.

Next, we will look at the function of aims and hypotheses in research and psychology.

Later, we will look at specific aims and hypotheses in research examples.

Finally, we will discuss the need and how explicit research aims objectives and hypotheses are implemented.

Difference Between Aims and Hypotheses: Psychology

When you write a research report, you should state the aim first and then the hypothesis.

The aim is a summary of the goal or purpose of the research.

The aim is a broad starting point that gets narrowed down into the hypothesis.

  • The hypothesis is a predictive, testable statement about what the researcher expects to find in the study.

Types of Hypotheses

Before we get into the types of hypotheses, let's quickly recap the hypotheses' components.

The independent variable (IV) is the factor that the researcher manipulates/ changes (this can be naturally occurring in some instances) and is theorised to be the cause of a phenomenon.

And the dependent variable (DV) is the factor that the researcher measures because they believe that the changes in the IV will affect the DV.

There are two types of hypotheses: null and alternative hypotheses.

A null hypothesis states that the independent variable does not influence the dependent variable. The null hypothesis states that changes/ manipulating the IV will not affect the DV.

Research scenario: Investigation of how test results affect sleep.

An example of a null hypothesis is there is no difference in recorded sleep time (dependent variable) between students who received good and poor grades (independent variable).

An alternative hypothesis states that the independent variable has an effect on the dependent variable. Often, it is the same (or very similar) to your research hypothesis.

Research scenario: Investigating how sleep deprivation affects performance on cognitive tests.

An alternative hypothesis may be that the less sleep students get (independent variable), the worse their performance will be on cognitive tests (dependent variable). Not sleep-deprived students will perform better on the Mini-Mental Status Examination test than sleep-deprived students.

Photograph of a bed in a bedroom. StudySmarter

The alternative hypothesis can be further sub-categorised into a one- or two-tailed hypothesis. A one-tailed hypothesis (also known as a directional hypothesis) suggests that the results can go one way, e.g. it may increase or decrease. And a two-tailed (also known as a non-directional hypothesis) is exactly the opposite; there are two ways the results could expectedly go.

An example of a two-tailed hypothesis is if you flip a coin, you could predict that it will land on either heads or tails.

Aims and Hypothesis in Research

In research, aims and hypotheses play major roles. They are the part of the research that sets you up for the rest of the study. Without strong research aims and hypotheses, your research will lack direction.

First, let's go over the function of research aims.

Research aims provide an overview of the research objective; this allows all researchers to be on the same page about the purpose of the research. Aims also describe why the research is needed and how it complements existing research in the field.

Duplicating research can sometimes be useful, but most times, researchers want to conduct their own new research.

Outside of the researchers, readers can then identify the research topic and whether it interests them.

The research aimed to examine the effects of sleep deprivation on test performance.

But what information do hypotheses provide?

Hypotheses identify the variables studied in an experiment. They describe expected results in terms of the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable. When readers see the hypothesis, they should know exactly what the researcher expected in the study's outcome (remember, sometimes the researcher can be wrong).

The hypothesis was that the less sleep a student gets (independent variable), the worse grades a student will achieve (dependent variable).

Typically, researchers use hypotheses for statistical tests such as hypothesis testing, which allows them to determine if the original predictions are correct. Hypotheses are helpful because the reader can quickly identify the variables , the expected results based on previous research, and how the experiment should measure these variables .

Hypotheses usually influence the research design and analysis used in conducting the research.

Psychological research must meet a standard for the psychological research community to accept it.

Components of Hypotheses

When writing research hypotheses, there are several essential things to consider, including:

The hypotheses must be clear and concise;

it must be easy to understand and not contain irrelevant details.

The researcher must predict what they expect to find based on reading previous research findings.

The researcher must explain how they arrived at their predictions, citing evidence from prior research.

The researcher must identify all variables they will study.

One study examined how sleep deprivation affects performance on cognitive tests. The hypothesis was to identify sleeping time as the independent variable and cognitive test scores as the dependent variable.

Additionally, the research must operationalise the hypotheses and describe how the variables will be measured.

When assessing cognitive abilities, the researcher should indicate how they will assess the cognitive skills. They could do so with a cognitive test, such as the Mini-Mental Status Examination scores.

Example Hypothesis

A hypothesis denotes a relationship between two variables, the independent and dependent variables. An example hypothesis is the more you sleep, the less tired you will feel.

Aims and Hypotheses: Psychology

Now that we understand the difference between aims and hypotheses, let's take a closer look at their function.

In psychology, aims and hypotheses function very similarly to other research fields. They set up the purpose of a study so that the researchers and readers understand its goals.

The aims establish the reasoning behind the study and why that specific topic is being researched. And the hypotheses share the researchers' expectations. It outlines what the researchers expect when the IV is manipulated.

Studies with well-defined aims and hypotheses allow the research to be more accessible. This means that a professional psychologist, a psychology student, or even someone who is simply curious about the topic can all read the research and understand its purpose.

Aims and Hypotheses in Research Example

As we have learned, aims and hypotheses are crucial in setting up successful research. They exist within every study and help outline the goals and outcomes the researchers expect. To further understand the aims and hypotheses in psychological research, let's look at a famous study – Asch's line experiment .

Solomon Asch conducted a study in 1951 about conformity . This study has become renowned for exposing the strong effects of conformity in a group setting. Asch put one participant in a room with seven strangers, people he said were other participants but were, in fact, confederates.

Confederates are hired actors who are told what to do in the experiment by the researcher.

The participants were tasked with trying to match one line to three other lines. Initially, the confederates would answer correctly, but as the trials continued, they all answered incorrectly. Would the participant still give the correct answer, or would they be swayed by conformity and be wrong?

Asch found that 74% of participants conformed at least once, even though they were obviously giving the wrong answer.

Photograph of a university lecture room. StudySmarter

In this experiment, the aim was to look at the effects of conformity. More specifically, Asch aimed to see how impactful groups' pressures are on an individual's conformity. Asch hypothesised that participants would conform to the group when the confederates answered incorrectly due to social pressure.

Since we know the study's outcome, we know that Asch stayed true to his aims and provided supporting evidence for his hypothesis.

Explicit Research Aims, Objectives, and Hypotheses

An explicit research necessity across all disciplines is the operalisation of variables. When talking about operationalising a variable or hypothesis, it means that the term is defined so clearly and succinctly that there is no confusion or any grey area concerning what it means.

When operationally defining variables, researchers need to not only define what the variable is but also how they will measure it. Operationally defined hypotheses not only include detailed descriptions of variables and the outcome but also the relationship between the variables.

Remember, when studies and their results are replicated, they increase in reliability. Researchers operationally defining variables and hypotheses help future researchers replicate their study without confusion. If you do not operationally define key terms of your research and no one can replicate it, is there even a purpose to doing the research at all?

While operationally defining variables and hypotheses might seem like a simple task, it is extremely important for a successful outcome.

Aims and Hypotheses - Key takeaways

  • For the scientific psychological community to accept the aim, the objective must explain why the research is needed and how it will expand our current knowledge.
  • The two types of hypotheses are null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis.
  • For the scientific community of psychologists to accept a hypothesis, it must identify all variables, which researchers must operationalise.

Flashcards in Aims and Hypotheses 28

What is the definition of aims?

What is the definition of a hypothesis? 

The hypothesis is a predictive, testable statement of what the researcher expects to find in the study.

What is the purpose of research aims?

The purpose of research aims are: 

  • provide a summary of what the research goal is 
  • describe why the research is needed and how it adds to existing research in the field
  • so that the readers can identify what the research topic is and of interest to them

How are hypotheses different from aims?

Hypotheses differ from aims because they are statements of the goals and purposes of the research. In contrast, hypotheses are predictive statements concerning expected results. 

What are the different types of hypotheses?

The types of hypotheses are:

  • Null hypothesis.
  • Alternative hypothesis.
  • Directional alternative hypothesis (one-tailed) or non-directional (two-tailed).

What type of hypothesis is the following statement: ‘There will be a difference between Mini-Mental Status Examination scores in students who were and were not sleep-deprived?  

Aims and Hypotheses

Learn with 28 Aims and Hypotheses flashcards in the free StudySmarter app

We have 14,000 flashcards about Dynamic Landscapes.

Already have an account? Log in

Frequently Asked Questions about Aims and Hypotheses

How to write aims and hypotheses?

When writing aims, researchers should summarise the research goal and purpose in a straightforward statement. Moreover, researchers must ensure that it is a predictive and testable statement when writing a hypothesis. This process should summarise the expected results of the study. 

What comes first, hypothesis or aims?

Researchers should write the aims first and then the hypothesis when writing research. 

What are the three types of hypotheses?

The three types of hypotheses are:

What is an aim in psychology?

An aim in psychology is a summary statement of the research's goal or purpose.

How are hypotheses different from aims and objectives?

Hypotheses differ from aims and objectives because aims are a general statement of the research's goals and purposes. In contrast, hypotheses explain precisely the predicted findings in terms of the independent and dependent variables. 

Test your knowledge with multiple choice flashcards

What type of hypothesis is the following statement: ‘There will be no difference in time recorded sleeping between students who received good and poor grades in their school report’?

What type of hypothesis is this statement ‘Students who were not sleep-deprived will have higher scores in the Mini-Mental Status Examination test than sleep-deprived students?

Aims and Hypotheses

Join the StudySmarter App and learn efficiently with millions of flashcards and more!

Keep learning, you are doing great.

Discover learning materials with the free StudySmarter app

1

About StudySmarter

StudySmarter is a globally recognized educational technology company, offering a holistic learning platform designed for students of all ages and educational levels. Our platform provides learning support for a wide range of subjects, including STEM, Social Sciences, and Languages and also helps students to successfully master various tests and exams worldwide, such as GCSE, A Level, SAT, ACT, Abitur, and more. We offer an extensive library of learning materials, including interactive flashcards, comprehensive textbook solutions, and detailed explanations. The cutting-edge technology and tools we provide help students create their own learning materials. StudySmarter’s content is not only expert-verified but also regularly updated to ensure accuracy and relevance.

Aims and Hypotheses

StudySmarter Editorial Team

Team Aims and Hypotheses Teachers

  • 9 minutes reading time
  • Checked by StudySmarter Editorial Team

Study anywhere. Anytime.Across all devices.

Create a free account to save this explanation..

Save explanations to your personalised space and access them anytime, anywhere!

By signing up, you agree to the Terms and Conditions and the Privacy Policy of StudySmarter.

Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.

Join over 22 million students in learning with our StudySmarter App

The first learning app that truly has everything you need to ace your exams in one place

  • Flashcards & Quizzes
  • AI Study Assistant
  • Study Planner
  • Smart Note-Taking

Join over 22 million students in learning with our StudySmarter App

Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

Saul Mcleod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Behaviorism, MRes, PhD, University off Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a skill psychology teacher with over 65 years of experience in further and higher education. He is been published in peer-reviewed journals, with that Journal of Clinical Psychology. Null Hypothesis: As Is It and How Is It Used in Investing.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Science

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Training

Olivia Guy-Evans is one journalist and assoziiert editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare or educational divider.

On This Page:

A investigation hypothesis, in its plural form “hypotheses,” is ampere specialized, testable prediction about the anticipated results of a study, established at its starts. It is a key component to the scientific manner .

Hypotheses connect theory to datas and guide aforementioned research process for expanding scientific understanding

Some keyboard points about hypotheses:

  • A theme expresses an expected pattern or relational. She connects the variables under investigation.
  • It exists stated by free, precise term before any data accumulation or analysis occurs. To produces the hypothesis testable.
  • A hypothesis must be falsifiable. Itp should be potential, same if unlikely in habit, to collect data that disconfirms rather than supports the hypothesis.
  • Hypotheses guide research. Scientists design studies into explicitly evaluate hypothesen about how nature works.
  • For a myth to be valid, to must be testable against empirical evidence. Of evidence can then confirm or disproves who testable prognoses.
  • Hypotheses are fully with background knowledge also observer, but go beyond what is before acknowledged to propose an explanation of how or why existence occurs. In biology, who void hypothesis is used up invalidate or reject a common belief And experimenter carries away of find which has aimed toward.
Predictions typically ascend from a thoroughgoing knowledge of the doing literature, curiosity about real-world problems or meanings, and integrating this to advance theory. They build on existing literature while providing new insight. 26 4 Setting the Hypotheses: Examples STAT 502.

Types of Research Hypothese

Alternative hypothesis.

The exploration hypothesis is frequency called the alternative or experimental hypothesis in experimental research.

It typically suggests a potential relationship between two key variables: the independent variable, which the researcher manipulates, and the depends variable, which is measured based on those changes.

One alternative hypothesis states a relationship exists between the two variables be studied (one variable affects the other).

A proof is a testable statement or forecasts regarding the your between two or more variables. It is ampere key component of aforementioned scientific method. Some key points about hypotheses:

  • A hypothesis shown an anticipated pattern or relationship. Itp relates the variables under investigations.
  • This is stated in clear, precise terms before any data collection or analysis happens. This makes the hypothesis demonstrable.
  • A hypothesis must be falsifiable. It should be possible, steady if highly in practice, on collect data that disconfirms rather than supports the hypothesis. A null hypothesis is a type of statistical hypothesis that proposes that no statistical meanings exists in a firm of give observations.
  • Hypotheses steer research. Scientists design studies to explicitly evaluate hypotheses about how nature works.
  • Important hypotheses lead to forecasting that can be tested empirically. The evidence can then confirm or disprove the testable predictions.
  • Hypotheses are informative by backgrounds general and observation, but go besides whichever lives already known to propose an explanation of how or mystery something occurs.

In summery, adenine hypothesis is a precise, testable statement in what researchers waiting to occur in a study and reasons. Hypotheses connect theory to details and guide the research process towards expanding scientific sympathy.

An experimental hypothesis projected what change(s) will occur in the depended variable when aforementioned independent variable is rigged.

It states that the results are not due at chance and are substantial in help the assumption being investigated.

The replacement hypothesis can be directional, indicating a specific direction of the effect, or non-directional, suggesting a difference excluding specifying its nature. It’s what researchers objective the support or demonstrate through their study.

Null Hypothesis

The void hypothesis declare no related exists between the two variables being studied (one variable can not affect the other). Here will remain no changes in the dependents varia due toward manipulating the independent variable.

To states results are due to chance and are not significant inches supporting the item being investigated.

The null hypothesis, positing don effect or relationship, is a foundational contrast in the research hypothesis include scientific ask. It establishes a baseline for statistical validation, promoting objectivity by initiating research since a neutral stance.

Many statistical methods are tailored to test the null hypothesis, determining an likelihood of observed ergebnis if not true effect exists.

This dual-hypothesis approach provides clarity, ensuring the research intentions are explicit, and fosters consistency across scientific degree, enhance the standardization and interpretability of research outcomes. Hypothesis Examples: Different Types inbound Science and Choose.

Nondirectional Hypothesis

A non-directional hypothesis, also known as a two-tailed hypothesis, forecast that there is a difference or ratio between two variable but does not specify the direction of this relationship.

It merely indicates that a change or execute will occur without predicting what group wants can higher or lower set.

For example, “There is a difference in performance between Bunch AN and Group B” is a non-directional hypothesis.

Directional Hypothesis

A antenna (one-tailed) hypothesis predicts the nature of the effect of the independent variable on that dependence total. It predicts in whatever direction of change will record place. (i.e., greater, smaller, get, more) The greater number in black plants in a region independent variable increases water.

It specifies whether one varia exists greater, lesser, or different from another, rather for just show that there’s a difference without specifying seine nature. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis Steps Examples.

For example, “Exercise increases weight loss” are ampere directional hypothesis.

research

Falsifiability

The Falsification Principle, proposed by Karl Popper , is a way a demarcating science from non-science. A suggests that required a theorizing or hypothesis to be considered research, it must be testable and irrefutable.

Falsifiability emphasizes that scientific claims shouldn’t just be confirmable but should also have the potential to be prove wrong.

It means that present should exist some potential evidence conversely experiment that could prove the proposition false.

However many confirming instances exist since a theory, it only takes single counter observation till distortion it. For example, the myth that “all swans are white,” can be falsified by observing a black swan. Coupled T-Test.

For Popper, science shall attempt to disprove adenine idea tend than attempt to continually provision evidence to support one research hypothesis.

Can an Hypothesis be Proven?

Hypotheses create probabilistic predictions. They state the expected outcome if a particular relationship exists. Not, a study result supporting one hypothesis does not definitively prove it is truthfully.

All studies have constraints. There may be unknown confounding factors or issues that limit an security of conclusions. Additional student may yield different results.

In scholarship, hypotheses can realistically merely be assist with some degree of confidence, no tested. The process of skill has to incrementally accumulate detection by and against hypothesized relationships in an ongoing pursuit the better models and explanations that best fit the empirical data. But assumptions remain open go revision or reaction if that is where the evidence leads.
  • Disproving a hypothesis is default. Solid disconfirmatory evidence will falsify a hypothesis and require transform or discarding it based on the evidence.
  • However, confirming evidence is always open till revision. Other instructions may account on who similar results, and additional or contradictory evidence may emerge over time.

We can not 781% prove the alternative hypothesis. Instead, we see is we can disprove, or reject the aught hypothesis.

While we reject the nothing hypothesis, this doesn’t mean that our alternative hypothesis is correct but shall support the alternative/experimental hypothesis.

Upon analysis of of results, an variant hypothesis can be rejected or supported, but information can not be proven at be correct. We must prevent any reference to results proving adenine theory as this implies 432% certainty, plus there exists forever a chance that evidence may exist which can refute a general. Null Hypothesis Examples Research Question, Zilch Hypothesis Does the vaccine prevent infections The vaccine does non affect the infection rate Does the new.

How to Record a Hypothesis

  • Identify variables . An researcher manipulates to independent variable press this dependent variable is the assured outcome.
  • Operationalized the variables being investigated . Operationalization regarding a hypothesis refers to the process away making the variables physically measurable or testable, e.g. if you are about for study aggression, you might count the number out punches given by participants.
  • Decide on a direction for your project . If there a evidence are to literature go support a specific effect of the independent variable on and dependents varies, write a directional (one-tailed) hypothesis. If there are limited or ambiguous findings inbound that literature regarding the effect of who standalone variable on the dependent variable, written a non-directional (two-tailed) hypothesis.
  • Make it Testable : Ensure your hypothesis can be tested through experimentation or observation. It should be possible to prove it false (principle of falsifiability).
  • Clear & concise language . A strong hypothesis is concise (typically one to two sentences long), and formulated through clear and easy language, ensuring it’s easily get and testable.

Consider a hypothesis many instructors might subscribe to: students work better upon Monday midnight than on Friday afternoon (IV=Day, DV= Standard for work).

Now, if we decide to study this by giving this same group of college ampere lesson on a Monday morning and a Friday afternoon furthermore then measuring to immediate recall of the material covered in each session, ours would end above with the following:

  • That alternative hypothesis states that students will recall significantly more about on a Donnerstag dawn than at one Weekday afternoon.
  • The empty hypothesis states that there will exist no significant difference included the amount retained on a Mondtag midday compared to a Friday p. Any gauge will be due to occasion or confounding factors.

Find Examples

  • Storages : Participants exposed to classical music during study sessions will recall more items from a tabbed less those who studying in silence.
  • Social Behaviourism : Individuals who frequently involved in social media use will report higher levels of perceived social isolation compared to those those application it infrequently.
  • Experimental Psychology : Children anybody engage in regular fantastic play have better problem-solving skills than those which don’t.
  • Clinical Psychology : Cognitive-behavioral therapy will be show effective in reducing symptoms of anxiety over a 6-month period compared to traditional talk therapy.
  • Cognitive Psychology : Individuals who multitask between various electronic devices will must shorter attention spans on concentrate tasks with those who single-task.
  • Dental Psychology : Patients who practice mindfulness meditation wishes experience lower layer of habitual pain compared to who who don’t meditate.
  • Organizational Psychology : Employees inside open-plan offices will report higher levels of stress than those by private offices.
  • Behavioral Psychology : Mouse rewarded with food after pressing a lever will push computers more commonly than rats who received no reward.

Mark Friendly, PDF & Email

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology in Education

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

Olivia Guy-Evans is adenine writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has up working in healthcare furthermore educational sectors.

research hypothesis example psychology

Editor-in-Chief by Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Mental, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 98 years of experience in further additionally higher education. His has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Relative Articles

Qualitative Data Coding

Research Methodology

Qualitative Data Coding

How Is a Focus Group?

What Is a Focus Group?

Cross-Cultural Research Methodology Is Psychology

Study Methodology

Cross-Cultural Researching Methodology In Psychology

What Is In Validity For Research?

What Is Internal Valid In Research?

What Is Face Validity In Research? Meaning & How To Measure

Research Methodology , Vital

What Is Face Validity Includes Find? Importance & How Until Measure

Criterion Validity: Definition & Examples

Research Methodology , Statistics

Criterion Validity: Definition & Examples

research hypothesis example psychology

  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar

IResearchNet

In scientific research, a hypothesis is a statement about  a  predicted  relationship  between  variables. A good research hypothesis can be formulated as an “if-then” statement:

  • If a child is exposed to the music of Mozart, then that child’s intelligence will increase.
  • If students learn a math lesson by interacting with a computer, then they will solve math problems more accurately than students who learn the same lesson by listening to a lecture.

Notice that a hypothesis is not a question. It is a statement,  a  prediction  that  requires  the  researcher to go out on a limb and say what he or she thinks will happen in a given situation. When stating a hypothesis, the researcher must run the risk of being wrong—a scientific hypothesis must be falsifiable.

Hypotheses come from many sources. Researchers are not all wildly creative people, but they do tend to be careful observers of the world around them. One’s own everyday observations can lead to the formulation of a hypothesis, as when a babysitter observes that “children who eat ice cream before bedtime have a harder time falling asleep.” That simple observation can lead to a formal hypothesis about the relationship between sugar consumption and sleep onset. A famous hypothesis in social psychology was generated from a news story, when a woman in New York City was murdered in full view of dozens of onlookers. Instead of simply shaking their heads in sadness, psychologists John Darley and Bibb Latané developed a hypothesis about the relationship between helping behavior and the number of bystanders present, and that hypothesis was  subsequently  supported  by  research.  This  type of reasoning from a specific case to a more general principle is called inductive logic.

Reading existing research and theory can also lead to the generation of hypotheses. Through the process of deductiv e logic, a general theory leads to the prediction of a specific effect or conclusion. For example, someone who is familiar with Piaget’s theories of human development might predict that “if a child is younger than the age of 12, then that child will be unable to solve an abstract reasoning problem.” Such a hypothesis could then be put to the test in systematic research.

Hypotheses can be either directional or nondirectional. A directional hypothesis states a specific prediction about the precise type of effect that a variable is expected to have on another variable—for example, “If the number of bystanders increases, then the probability of any given bystander rendering help decreases.” A nondirectional hypothesis states that a relationship will exist between two variables, but it is not specific about the nature of that relationship: “If the number of bystanders increases, then the probability of any given bystander rendering help will change.” This type of hypothesis can be confirmed if the probability of help increases or if it decreases. Nondirectional hypotheses are useful in the early stages of research in a given area, when the researcher may not have enough information  to  make  a  more  specific  prediction. A nondirectional hypothesis is still falsifiable, however, if the data suggest that there is no systematic relationship between the variables after all.

Whether directional or nondirectional, a good research hypothesis must ultimately be objectively testable. Before actually turning a hypothesis into a study, the researcher must develop operational definitions of the variables stated in the hypothesis. If the hypothesis postulates that “If a child is exposed to the music of Mozart, then that child’s intelligence will increase,” then the researcher must define what specifically is meant by “child” (a person under the age of ?), by “intelligence” (a score on a particular standardized test, perhaps), and what it means to be “exposed” to the music of Mozart (Which compositions by Mozart? For how long? Played how loudly?). Thus, the development of the hypothesis is only the beginning of the process of psychological research.

References:

  • Beins, C. (2004). Research methods: A tool for life. Boston: Pearson.
  • Dunn, D.  S.  (1999).   The  practical  researcher.   Boston: McGraw-Hill.
  • Smith, A., & Davis, S. F. (2004). The psychologist as detective (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  • Stockburger, W. (n.d.). Hypothesis testing . Retrieved from http://www.psychstat.smsu.edu/introbook/SBK18.htm
  • Trochim, W. (2002). Research methods knowledge base . Retrieved from http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/index.htm

The Three Most Common Types of Hypotheses

In this post, I discuss three of the most common hypotheses in psychology research, and what statistics are often used to test them.

  • Post author By sean
  • Post date September 28, 2013
  • 37 Comments on The Three Most Common Types of Hypotheses

research hypothesis example psychology

Simple main effects (i.e., X leads to Y) are usually not going to get you published. Main effects can be exciting in the early stages of research to show the existence of a new effect, but as a field matures the types of questions that scientists are trying to answer tend to become more nuanced and specific.  In this post, I’ll briefly describe the three most common kinds of hypotheses that expand upon simple main effects – at least, the most common ones I’ve seen in my research career in psychology – as well as providing some resources to help you learn about how to test these hypotheses using statistics.

Incremental Validity

“Can X predict Y over and above other important predictors?”

Inc_Validity

This is probably the simplest of the three hypotheses I propose. Basically, you attempt to rule out potential confounding variables by controlling for them in your analysis.  We do this because (in many cases) our predictor variables are correlated with each other. This is undesirable from a statistical perspective, but is common with real data. The idea is that we want to see if X can predict unique variance in Y over and above the other variables you include.

In terms of analysis, you are probably going to use some variation of multiple regression or partial correlations.  For example, in my own work I’ve shown in the past that friendship intimacy as coded from autobiographical narratives can predict concern for the next generation over and above numerous other variables, such as optimism, depression, and relationship status ( Mackinnon et al., 2011 ).

“Under what conditions does X lead to Y?”

Of the three techniques I describe, moderation is probably the most tricky to understand.  Essentially, it proposes that the size of a relationship between two variables changes depending upon the value of a third variable, known as a “moderator.”  For example, in the diagram below you might find a simple main effect that is moderated by sex. That is, the relationship is stronger for women than for men:

moderation

With moderation, it is important to note that the moderating variable can be a category (e.g., sex) or it can be a continuous variable (e.g., scores on a personality questionnaire).  When a moderator is continuous, usually you’re making statements like: “As the value of the moderator increases, the relationship between X and Y also increases.”

“Does X predict M, which in turn predicts Y?”

We might know that X leads to Y, but a mediation hypothesis proposes a mediating, or intervening variable. That is, X leads to M, which in turn leads to Y.  In the diagram below I use a different way of visually representing things consistent with how people typically report things when using path analysis.

Mediation

I use mediation a lot in my own research. For example, I’ve published data suggesting the relationship between perfectionism and depression is mediated by relationship conflict ( Mackinnon et al., 2012 ). That is, perfectionism leads to increased conflict, which in turn leads to heightened depression. Another way of saying this is that perfectionism has an indirect effect on depression through conflict.

Helpful links to get you started testing these hypotheses

Depending on the nature of your data, there are multiple ways to address each of these hypotheses using statistics. They can also be combined together (e.g., mediated moderation). Nonetheless, a core understanding of these three hypotheses and how to analyze them using statistics is essential for any researcher in the social or health sciences.  Below are a few links that might help you get started:

Are you a little rusty with multiple regression? The basics of this technique are required for most common tests of these hypotheses. You might check out this guide as a helpful resource:

https://statistics.laerd.com/spss-tutorials/multiple-regression-using-spss-statistics.php

David Kenny’s Mediation Website provides an excellent overview of mediation and moderation for the beginner.

http://davidakenny.net/cm/mediate.htm

http://davidakenny.net/cm/moderation.htm

Preacher and Haye’s INDIRECT Macro is a great, easy way to implement mediation in SPSS software, and their MODPROBE macro is a useful tool for testing moderation.

http://afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplus-macros-and-code.html

If you want to graph the results of your moderation analyses, the excel calculators provided on Jeremy Dawson’s webpage are fantastic, easy-to-use tools:

http://www.jeremydawson.co.uk/slopes.htm

  • Tags mediation , moderation , regression , tutorial

37 replies on “The Three Most Common Types of Hypotheses”

I want to see clearly the three types of hypothesis

Thanks for your information. I really like this

Thank you so much, writing up my masters project now and wasn’t sure whether one of my variables was mediating or moderating….Much clearer now.

Thank you for simplified presentation. It is clearer to me now than ever before.

Thank you. Concise and clear

hello there

I would like to ask about mediation relationship: If I have three variables( X-M-Y)how many hypotheses should I write down? Should I have 2 or 3? In other words, should I have hypotheses for the mediating relationship? What about questions and objectives? Should be 3? Thank you.

Hi Osama. It’s really a stylistic thing. You could write it out as 3 separate hypotheses (X -> Y; X -> M; M -> Y) or you could just write out one mediation hypotheses “X will have an indirect effect on Y through M.” Usually, I’d write just the 1 because it conserves space, but either would be appropriate.

Hi Sean, according to the three steps model (Dudley, Benuzillo and Carrico, 2004; Pardo and Román, 2013)., we can test hypothesis of mediator variable in three steps: (X -> Y; X -> M; X and M -> Y). Then, we must use the Sobel test to make sure that the effect is significant after using the mediator variable.

Yes, but this is older advice. Best practice now is to calculate an indirect effect and use bootstrapping, rather than the causal steps approach and the more out-dated Sobel test. I’d recommend reading Hayes (2018) book for more info:

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed). Guilford Publications.

Hi! It’s been really helpful but I still don’t know how to formulate the hypothesis with my mediating variable.

I have one dependent variable DV which is formed by DV1 and DV2, then I have MV (mediating variable), and then 2 independent variables IV1, and IV2.

How many hypothesis should I write? I hope you can help me 🙂

Thank you so much!!

If I’m understanding you correctly, I guess 2 mediation hypotheses:

IV1 –> Med –> DV1&2 IV2 –> Med –> DV1&2

Thank you so much for your quick answer! ^^

Could you help me formulate my research question? English is not my mother language and I have trouble choosing the right words. My x = psychopathy y = aggression m = deficis in emotion recognition

thank you in advance

I have mediator and moderator how should I make my hypothesis

Can you have a negative partial effect? IV – M – DV. That is my M will have negative effect on the DV – e.g Social media usage (M) will partial negative mediate the relationship between father status (IV) and social connectedness (DV)?

Thanks in advance

Hi Ashley. Yes, this is possible, but often it means you have a condition known as “inconsistent mediation” which isn’t usually desirable. See this entry on David Kenny’s page:

Or look up “inconsistent mediation” in this reference:

MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593-614.

This is very interesting presentation. i love it.

This is very interesting and educative. I love it.

Hello, you mentioned that for the moderator, it changes the relationship between iv and dv depending on its strength. How would one describe a situation where if the iv is high iv and dv relationship is opposite from when iv is low. And then a 3rd variable maybe the moderator increases dv when iv is low and decreases dv when iv is high.

This isn’t problematic for moderation. Moderation just proposes that the magnitude of the relationship changes as levels of the moderator changes. If the sign flips, probably the original relationship was small. Sometimes people call this a “cross-over” effect, but really, it’s nothing special and can happen in any moderation analysis.

i want to use an independent variable as moderator after this i will have 3 independent variable and 1 dependent variable…. my confusion is do i need to have some past evidence of the X variable moderate the relationship of Y independent variable and Z dependent variable.

Dear Sean It is really helpful as my research model will use mediation. Because I still face difficulty in developing hyphothesis, can you give examples ? Thank you

Hi! is it possible to have all three pathways negative? My regression analysis showed significant negative relationships between x to y, x to m and m to y.

Hi, I have 1 independent variable, 1 dependent variable and 4 mediating variable May I know how many hypothesis should I develop?

Hello I have 4 IV , 1 mediating Variable and 1 DV

My model says that 4 IVs when mediated by 1MV leads to 1 Dv

Pls tell me how to set the hypothesis for mediation

Hi I have 4 IVs ,2 Mediating Variables , 1DV and 3 Outcomes (criterion variables).

Pls can u tell me how many hypotheses to set.

Thankyou in advance

I am in fact happy to read this webpage posts which carries tons of useful information, thanks for providing such data.

I see you don’t monetize savvystatistics.com, don’t waste your traffic, you can earn additional bucks every month with new monetization method. This is the best adsense alternative for any type of website (they approve all websites), for more info simply search in gooogle: murgrabia’s tools

what if the hypothesis and moderator significant in regrestion and insgificant in moderation?

Thank you so much!! Your slide on the mediator variable let me understand!

Very informative material. The author has used very clear language and I would recommend this for any student of research/

Hi Sean, thanks for the nice material. I have a question: for the second type of hypothesis, you state “That is, the relationship is stronger for men than for women”. Based on the illustration, wouldn’t the opposite be true?

Yes, your right! I updated the post to fix the typo, thank you!

I have 3 independent variable one mediator and 2 dependant variable how many hypothesis I have 2 write?

Sounds like 6 mediation hypotheses total:

X1 -> M -> Y1 X2 -> M -> Y1 X3 -> M -> Y1 X1 -> M -> Y2 X2 -> M -> Y2 X3 -> M -> Y2

Clear explanation! Thanks!

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Examples

Research Question and Hypothesis

Ai generator.

research hypothesis example psychology

Navigating the intricacies of research begins with crafting well-defined research questions and hypothesis statements. These essential components guide the entire research process, shaping investigations and analyses. In this comprehensive guide, explore the art of formulating research questions and hypothesis statements. Learn how to create focused, inquiry-driven questions and construct research hypothesis statements that capture the essence of your study. Unveil examples and invaluable tips to enhance your research endeavors.

What is an example of a Research Question and Hypothesis Statement?

Research Question: How does regular exercise impact the mental well-being of college students?

Hypothesis Statement: College students who engage in regular exercise experience improved mental well-being compared to those who do not exercise regularly.

In this example, the research question focuses on the relationship between exercise and mental well-being among college students. The hypothesis statement predicts a specific outcome, stating that there will be a positive impact on mental well-being for those who exercise regularly. The hypothesis guides the research process and provides a clear expectation for the study’s results.

100 Research Question and Hypothesis Statement Examples

Research Question and Hypothesis Statement Examples

Size: 366 KB

Quantitative Research Question and Hypothesis Statement Examples

In quantitative research, researchers aim to collect and analyze numerical data to answer specific research questions. A quantitative research question is designed to be measurable and testable, and it often involves examining the relationship between variables. The corresponding hypothesis statement predicts the expected outcome of the research based on previous knowledge or theories.

Psychology Research Question and Hypothesis Statement Examples

Psychology is the scientific study of human behavior and mental processes. Psychology research questions delve into various aspects of human behavior, cognition, emotion, and more. These questions are designed to gain a deeper understanding of psychological phenomena. Hypothesis statements for psychology hypothesis  research predict how certain factors or variables might influence human behavior or mental processes.

Testable Research Question and Hypothesis Statement Examples

Testable research questions are formulated in a way that allows them to be tested through empirical observation or experimentation. These questions are often used in scientific and experimental research to investigate cause-and-effect relationships between variables. The corresponding hypothesis statements propose an expected outcome based on the variables being studied and the conditions of the experiment.

Is the Hypothesis Statement and Research Question Statement the Same Thing?

The hypothesis statement and research question statement are closely related but not the same. Both play crucial roles in research, but they serve distinct purposes.

  • Research Question Statement : A research question is a clear and concise inquiry that outlines the specific aspect of a topic you want to investigate. It is often expressed as an interrogative sentence and helps guide your research by focusing on a particular area of interest.
  • Hypothesis Statement : A hypothesis is a testable statement that predicts the relationship between variables. It’s based on existing knowledge or theories and proposes an expected outcome of your research. Hypotheses are formulated for experimental research and provide a basis for collecting and analyzing data.

How Do You State a Research Question and Hypothesis?

Research question :.

  • Identify the topic of interest.
  • Specify the aspect you want to explore.
  • Frame the question as a clear and concise interrogative sentence.
  • Ensure the question is researchable and not too broad or too narrow.

Hypothesis Statement :

  • Identify the variables involved (independent and dependent).
  • Formulate a prediction about their relationship.
  • Use clear language and avoid ambiguity.
  • Write it as a declarative statement.

How Do You Write a Research Question and Hypothesis Statement? – A Step by Step Guide

  • Identify the Topic : Choose a specific topic that interests you and is relevant to your field of study.
  • Background Research : Gather information about existing research related to your topic. This helps you understand what’s already known and identify gaps or areas for exploration.
  • Formulate the Research Question : Decide what aspect of the topic you want to investigate. Frame a clear, focused, and concise research question.
  • Identify Variables : Determine the independent and dependent variables in your research question. The independent variable is what you manipulate, and the dependent variable is what you measure.
  • Formulate the Hypothesis : Write a testable hypothesis that predicts the expected outcome based on the relationship between the variables.
  • Consider Null Hypothesis : Formulate a null hypothesis that states no relationship exists between the variables. This provides a baseline for comparison.

Tips for Writing Research Question and Hypothesis

  • Keep both the research question and hypothesis concise and specific.
  • Ensure they are testable and can be investigated through research.
  • Use clear language that accurately conveys your intentions.
  • Base your hypothesis on existing knowledge or theories.
  • Align the research question and hypothesis with the scope of your study.
  • Revise and refine your statements based on feedback and further research.

Remember, both research questions and hypotheses play essential roles in guiding your research and framing the investigation’s purpose and expected outcomes.

Twitter

Text prompt

  • Instructive
  • Professional

10 Examples of Public speaking

20 Examples of Gas lighting

Chi-Square (Χ²) Test & How To Calculate Formula Equation

Benjamin Frimodig

Science Expert

B.A., History and Science, Harvard University

Ben Frimodig is a 2021 graduate of Harvard College, where he studied the History of Science.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Saul Mcleod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

On This Page:

Chi-square (χ2) is used to test hypotheses about the distribution of observations into categories with no inherent ranking.

What Is a Chi-Square Statistic?

The Chi-square test (pronounced Kai) looks at the pattern of observations and will tell us if certain combinations of the categories occur more frequently than we would expect by chance, given the total number of times each category occurred.

It looks for an association between the variables. We cannot use a correlation coefficient to look for the patterns in this data because the categories often do not form a continuum.

There are three main types of Chi-square tests, tests of goodness of fit, the test of independence, and the test for homogeneity. All three tests rely on the same formula to compute a test statistic.

These tests function by deciphering relationships between observed sets of data and theoretical or “expected” sets of data that align with the null hypothesis.

What is a Contingency Table?

Contingency tables (also known as two-way tables) are grids in which Chi-square data is organized and displayed. They provide a basic picture of the interrelation between two variables and can help find interactions between them.

In contingency tables, one variable and each of its categories are listed vertically, and the other variable and each of its categories are listed horizontally.

Additionally, including column and row totals, also known as “marginal frequencies,” will help facilitate the Chi-square testing process.

In order for the Chi-square test to be considered trustworthy, each cell of your expected contingency table must have a value of at least five.

Each Chi-square test will have one contingency table representing observed counts (see Fig. 1) and one contingency table representing expected counts (see Fig. 2).

contingency table representing observed counts

Figure 1. Observed table (which contains the observed counts).

To obtain the expected frequencies for any cell in any cross-tabulation in which the two variables are assumed independent, multiply the row and column totals for that cell and divide the product by the total number of cases in the table.

contingency table representing observed counts

Figure 2. Expected table (what we expect the two-way table to look like if the two categorical variables are independent).

To decide if our calculated value for χ2 is significant, we also need to work out the degrees of freedom for our contingency table using the following formula: df= (rows – 1) x (columns – 1).

Formula Calculation

chi-squared-equation

Calculate the chi-square statistic (χ2) by completing the following steps:

  • Calculate the expected frequencies and the observed frequencies.
  • For each observed number in the table, subtract the corresponding expected number (O — E).
  • Square the difference (O —E)².
  • Divide the squares obtained for each cell in the table by the expected number for that cell (O – E)² / E.
  • Sum all the values for (O – E)² / E. This is the chi-square statistic.
  • Calculate the degrees of freedom for the contingency table using the following formula; df= (rows – 1) x (columns – 1).

Once we have calculated the degrees of freedom (df) and the chi-squared value (χ2), we can use the χ2 table (often at the back of a statistics book) to check if our value for χ2 is higher than the critical value given in the table. If it is, then our result is significant at the level given.

Interpretation

The chi-square statistic tells you how much difference exists between the observed count in each table cell to the counts you would expect if there were no relationship at all in the population.

Small Chi-Square Statistic: If the chi-square statistic is small and the p-value is large (usually greater than 0.05), this often indicates that the observed frequencies in the sample are close to what would be expected under the null hypothesis.

The null hypothesis usually states no association between the variables being studied or that the observed distribution fits the expected distribution.

In theory, if the observed and expected values were equal (no difference), then the chi-square statistic would be zero — but this is unlikely to happen in real life.

Large Chi-Square Statistic : If the chi-square statistic is large and the p-value is small (usually less than 0.05), then the conclusion is often that the data does not fit the model well, i.e., the observed and expected values are significantly different. This often leads to the rejection of the null hypothesis.

How to Report

To report a chi-square output in an APA-style results section, always rely on the following template:

χ2 ( degrees of freedom , N = sample size ) = chi-square statistic value , p = p value .

chi-squared-spss output

In the case of the above example, the results would be written as follows:

A chi-square test of independence showed that there was a significant association between gender and post-graduation education plans, χ2 (4, N = 101) = 54.50, p < .001.

APA Style Rules

  • Do not use a zero before a decimal when the statistic cannot be greater than 1 (proportion, correlation, level of statistical significance).
  • Report exact p values to two or three decimals (e.g., p = .006, p = .03).
  • However, report p values less than .001 as “ p < .001.”
  • Put a space before and after a mathematical operator (e.g., minus, plus, greater than, less than, equals sign).
  • Do not repeat statistics in both the text and a table or figure.

p -value Interpretation

You test whether a given χ2 is statistically significant by testing it against a table of chi-square distributions , according to the number of degrees of freedom for your sample, which is the number of categories minus 1. The chi-square assumes that you have at least 5 observations per category.

If you are using SPSS then you will have an expected p -value.

For a chi-square test, a p-value that is less than or equal to the .05 significance level indicates that the observed values are different to the expected values.

Thus, low p-values (p< .05) indicate a likely difference between the theoretical population and the collected sample. You can conclude that a relationship exists between the categorical variables.

Remember that p -values do not indicate the odds that the null hypothesis is true but rather provide the probability that one would obtain the sample distribution observed (or a more extreme distribution) if the null hypothesis was true.

A level of confidence necessary to accept the null hypothesis can never be reached. Therefore, conclusions must choose to either fail to reject the null or accept the alternative hypothesis, depending on the calculated p-value.

The four steps below show you how to analyze your data using a chi-square goodness-of-fit test in SPSS (when you have hypothesized that you have equal expected proportions).

Step 1 : Analyze > Nonparametric Tests > Legacy Dialogs > Chi-square… on the top menu as shown below:

Step 2 : Move the variable indicating categories into the “Test Variable List:” box.

Step 3 : If you want to test the hypothesis that all categories are equally likely, click “OK.”

Step 4 : Specify the expected count for each category by first clicking the “Values” button under “Expected Values.”

Step 5 : Then, in the box to the right of “Values,” enter the expected count for category one and click the “Add” button. Now enter the expected count for category two and click “Add.” Continue in this way until all expected counts have been entered.

Step 6 : Then click “OK.”

The four steps below show you how to analyze your data using a chi-square test of independence in SPSS Statistics.

Step 1 : Open the Crosstabs dialog (Analyze > Descriptive Statistics > Crosstabs).

Step 2 : Select the variables you want to compare using the chi-square test. Click one variable in the left window and then click the arrow at the top to move the variable. Select the row variable and the column variable.

Step 3 : Click Statistics (a new pop-up window will appear). Check Chi-square, then click Continue.

Step 4 : (Optional) Check the box for Display clustered bar charts.

Step 5 : Click OK.

Goodness-of-Fit Test

The Chi-square goodness of fit test is used to compare a randomly collected sample containing a single, categorical variable to a larger population.

This test is most commonly used to compare a random sample to the population from which it was potentially collected.

The test begins with the creation of a null and alternative hypothesis. In this case, the hypotheses are as follows:

Null Hypothesis (Ho) : The null hypothesis (Ho) is that the observed frequencies are the same (except for chance variation) as the expected frequencies. The collected data is consistent with the population distribution.

Alternative Hypothesis (Ha) : The collected data is not consistent with the population distribution.

The next step is to create a contingency table that represents how the data would be distributed if the null hypothesis were exactly correct.

The sample’s overall deviation from this theoretical/expected data will allow us to draw a conclusion, with a more severe deviation resulting in smaller p-values.

Test for Independence

The Chi-square test for independence looks for an association between two categorical variables within the same population.

Unlike the goodness of fit test, the test for independence does not compare a single observed variable to a theoretical population but rather two variables within a sample set to one another.

The hypotheses for a Chi-square test of independence are as follows:

Null Hypothesis (Ho) : There is no association between the two categorical variables in the population of interest.

Alternative Hypothesis (Ha) : There is no association between the two categorical variables in the population of interest.

The next step is to create a contingency table of expected values that reflects how a data set that perfectly aligns the null hypothesis would appear.

The simplest way to do this is to calculate the marginal frequencies of each row and column; the expected frequency of each cell is equal to the marginal frequency of the row and column that corresponds to a given cell in the observed contingency table divided by the total sample size.

Test for Homogeneity

The Chi-square test for homogeneity is organized and executed exactly the same as the test for independence.

The main difference to remember between the two is that the test for independence looks for an association between two categorical variables within the same population, while the test for homogeneity determines if the distribution of a variable is the same in each of several populations (thus allocating population itself as the second categorical variable).

Null Hypothesis (Ho) : There is no difference in the distribution of a categorical variable for several populations or treatments.

Alternative Hypothesis (Ha) : There is a difference in the distribution of a categorical variable for several populations or treatments.

The difference between these two tests can be a bit tricky to determine, especially in the practical applications of a Chi-square test. A reliable rule of thumb is to determine how the data was collected.

If the data consists of only one random sample with the observations classified according to two categorical variables, it is a test for independence. If the data consists of more than one independent random sample, it is a test for homogeneity.

What is the chi-square test?

The Chi-square test is a non-parametric statistical test used to determine if there’s a significant association between two or more categorical variables in a sample.

It works by comparing the observed frequencies in each category of a cross-tabulation with the frequencies expected under the null hypothesis, which assumes there is no relationship between the variables.

This test is often used in fields like biology, marketing, sociology, and psychology for hypothesis testing.

What does chi-square tell you?

The Chi-square test informs whether there is a significant association between two categorical variables. Suppose the calculated Chi-square value is above the critical value from the Chi-square distribution.

In that case, it suggests a significant relationship between the variables, rejecting the null hypothesis of no association.

How to calculate chi-square?

To calculate the Chi-square statistic, follow these steps:

1. Create a contingency table of observed frequencies for each category.

2. Calculate expected frequencies for each category under the null hypothesis.

3. Compute the Chi-square statistic using the formula: Χ² = Σ [ (O_i – E_i)² / E_i ], where O_i is the observed frequency and E_i is the expected frequency.

4. Compare the calculated statistic with the critical value from the Chi-square distribution to draw a conclusion.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Related Articles

Exploratory Data Analysis

Exploratory Data Analysis

What Is Face Validity In Research? Importance & How To Measure

Research Methodology , Statistics

What Is Face Validity In Research? Importance & How To Measure

Criterion Validity: Definition & Examples

Criterion Validity: Definition & Examples

Convergent Validity: Definition and Examples

Convergent Validity: Definition and Examples

Content Validity in Research: Definition & Examples

Content Validity in Research: Definition & Examples

Construct Validity In Psychology Research

Construct Validity In Psychology Research

research hypothesis example psychology

Live revision! Join us for our free exam revision livestreams Watch now →

Reference Library

Collections

  • See what's new
  • All Resources
  • Student Resources
  • Assessment Resources
  • Teaching Resources
  • CPD Courses
  • Livestreams

Study notes, videos, interactive activities and more!

Psychology news, insights and enrichment

Currated collections of free resources

Browse resources by topic

  • All Psychology Resources

Resource Selections

Currated lists of resources

Hypothesis Testing

Hypothesis testing is an important feature of science, as this is how theories are developed and modified. A good theory should generate testable predictions (hypotheses), and if research fails to support the hypotheses, then this suggests that the theory needs to be modified in some way.

  • Share on Facebook
  • Share on Twitter
  • Share by Email

Research Methods: MCQ Revision Test 1 for AQA A Level Psychology

Topic Videos

Example Answers for Research Methods: A Level Psychology, Paper 2, June 2018 (AQA)

Exam Support

A Level Psychology Topic Quiz - Research Methods

Quizzes & Activities

Our subjects

  • › Criminology
  • › Economics
  • › Geography
  • › Health & Social Care
  • › Psychology
  • › Sociology
  • › Teaching & learning resources
  • › Student revision workshops
  • › Online student courses
  • › CPD for teachers
  • › Livestreams
  • › Teaching jobs

Boston House, 214 High Street, Boston Spa, West Yorkshire, LS23 6AD Tel: 01937 848885

  • › Contact us
  • › Terms of use
  • › Privacy & cookies

© 2002-2024 Tutor2u Limited. Company Reg no: 04489574. VAT reg no 816865400.

Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, automatically generate references for free.

  • Knowledge Base
  • Methodology
  • How to Write a Strong Hypothesis | Guide & Examples

How to Write a Strong Hypothesis | Guide & Examples

Published on 6 May 2022 by Shona McCombes .

A hypothesis is a statement that can be tested by scientific research. If you want to test a relationship between two or more variables, you need to write hypotheses before you start your experiment or data collection.

Table of contents

What is a hypothesis, developing a hypothesis (with example), hypothesis examples, frequently asked questions about writing hypotheses.

A hypothesis states your predictions about what your research will find. It is a tentative answer to your research question that has not yet been tested. For some research projects, you might have to write several hypotheses that address different aspects of your research question.

A hypothesis is not just a guess – it should be based on existing theories and knowledge. It also has to be testable, which means you can support or refute it through scientific research methods (such as experiments, observations, and statistical analysis of data).

Variables in hypotheses

Hypotheses propose a relationship between two or more variables . An independent variable is something the researcher changes or controls. A dependent variable is something the researcher observes and measures.

In this example, the independent variable is exposure to the sun – the assumed cause . The dependent variable is the level of happiness – the assumed effect .

Prevent plagiarism, run a free check.

Step 1: ask a question.

Writing a hypothesis begins with a research question that you want to answer. The question should be focused, specific, and researchable within the constraints of your project.

Step 2: Do some preliminary research

Your initial answer to the question should be based on what is already known about the topic. Look for theories and previous studies to help you form educated assumptions about what your research will find.

At this stage, you might construct a conceptual framework to identify which variables you will study and what you think the relationships are between them. Sometimes, you’ll have to operationalise more complex constructs.

Step 3: Formulate your hypothesis

Now you should have some idea of what you expect to find. Write your initial answer to the question in a clear, concise sentence.

Step 4: Refine your hypothesis

You need to make sure your hypothesis is specific and testable. There are various ways of phrasing a hypothesis, but all the terms you use should have clear definitions, and the hypothesis should contain:

  • The relevant variables
  • The specific group being studied
  • The predicted outcome of the experiment or analysis

Step 5: Phrase your hypothesis in three ways

To identify the variables, you can write a simple prediction in if … then form. The first part of the sentence states the independent variable and the second part states the dependent variable.

In academic research, hypotheses are more commonly phrased in terms of correlations or effects, where you directly state the predicted relationship between variables.

If you are comparing two groups, the hypothesis can state what difference you expect to find between them.

Step 6. Write a null hypothesis

If your research involves statistical hypothesis testing , you will also have to write a null hypothesis. The null hypothesis is the default position that there is no association between the variables. The null hypothesis is written as H 0 , while the alternative hypothesis is H 1 or H a .

Hypothesis testing is a formal procedure for investigating our ideas about the world using statistics. It is used by scientists to test specific predictions, called hypotheses , by calculating how likely it is that a pattern or relationship between variables could have arisen by chance.

A hypothesis is not just a guess. It should be based on existing theories and knowledge. It also has to be testable, which means you can support or refute it through scientific research methods (such as experiments, observations, and statistical analysis of data).

A research hypothesis is your proposed answer to your research question. The research hypothesis usually includes an explanation (‘ x affects y because …’).

A statistical hypothesis, on the other hand, is a mathematical statement about a population parameter. Statistical hypotheses always come in pairs: the null and alternative hypotheses. In a well-designed study , the statistical hypotheses correspond logically to the research hypothesis.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the ‘Cite this Scribbr article’ button to automatically add the citation to our free Reference Generator.

McCombes, S. (2022, May 06). How to Write a Strong Hypothesis | Guide & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved 7 June 2024, from https://www.scribbr.co.uk/research-methods/hypothesis-writing/

Is this article helpful?

Shona McCombes

Shona McCombes

Other students also liked, operationalisation | a guide with examples, pros & cons, what is a conceptual framework | tips & examples, a quick guide to experimental design | 5 steps & examples.

Elliot Murphy Ph.D.

How the Brain Processes Different Components of Language

Moving beyond neural localization of language..

Posted May 28, 2024 | Reviewed by Devon Frye

In a new article appearing in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , researchers at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, led by Cory Shain and Hope Kean, explore how the human brain shows signatures for linguistic composition . This refers to the ability we have to put together individual units of meaning into larger units, typically hierarchically organized.

The research is an excellent example of innovative neuroscience methods being paired with thoughtful and critical linguistic hypotheses to deliver novel insights on the question: “Which parts of the brain care about different core components of language?”

Shain and colleagues discovered an intricate profile of distributed sensitivity to both syntax and semantics throughout a broad frontotemporal network. This spanned from the posterior and middle lateral temporal cortex to the inferior frontal cortex.

The main take-home message here is that language network regions do not display binary sensitivity to either words, grammar, or larger meanings built from multiple words; rather, they vary in their sensitivity to at least two of these features.

They make the important conclusion that the “internal specialization” for language “is primarily a matter of degree rather than kind.” In addition, they practice something that should be standard across the field: they provide a Table with full definitions of working concepts and a Figure plotting a full schema of possible neural response profiles based on various hypothetical effects.

The authors show that major language network nodes display different levels of sensitivity to syntax, "combinatorial semantics" (referring to the types of complex meanings we can only derive from multi-word phrases, as opposed to individual words), and lexicality (referring to our ability to judge whether a string of letter forms a real word or not), rather than one fixed region being selective for a specific component.

Cortical Mosaics for Linguistic Structure

The authors found that posterior temporal and inferior frontal language sites showed a length effect in so-called "Jabberwocky" sentences (i.e., sentences in which all the "content words," like nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs, have been replaced with fake words) for linguistic structural context, and all language areas showed at least some effect of lexicality (i.e., a difference for real words vs. fake words).

This is in line with recent ideas about a "cortical mosaic" architecture for linguistic structure within overlapping portions of posterior temporal and inferior frontal cortices for processing demands that bias syntactic and semantic computations, whereby, for example, effects of composition can be found within a narrow strip of tissue within the broader lexicality-sensitive cortical sites (a spatial mosaic), or where different demands of sentence-level inferential semantics can be detected over closely overlapping temporal windows within a small area of cortex (a spatiotemporal mosaic).

Shain and colleagues also found that the posterior temporal cortex uniquely exhibits equal sensitivity to syntactic structure and combinatorial semantics, with or without lexical content. This potentially points to this region as a site heavily involved in linguistic structure-building.

This provides strong evidence that the brain’s language hubs are not monolithic in their interest in specific components of linguistic structure—meaning it's not syntax over here and semantics over there, as has been claimed by some of the field’s most influential researchers such as Friederici, Duffau, and Hagoort. Instead, it points to a kind of mosaic-like architecture whereby syntax and semantics are closely intermingled, but different regions exhibit various biases for processing loads and strategies.

Someone Must Extinguish Thy Flame: Finding Signatures of Syntax

There is a qualification to be made about the hypothesis presented by many authors before Shain and colleagues, and replicated in their study, that “syntactic hubs” would be unveiled via “identical responses” for real sentences and Jabberwocky sentences. Lexico-semantic modulations inevitably tax syntactic processes , given the centrality of lexical syntax in driving and initiating phrase structure formation and, subsequently, long-distance dependency formation.

As such, there are some core syntactic processes certainly captured in both real sentences and Jabberwocky sentences, such as hierarchical depth. But even here, as Shain and colleagues mention, there are certain properties of lexical meaning that can be inferred by specific properties of pseudowords, and every phrase structure is built out of properties such as lexical selection criteria (pseudowords do not select for anything).

research hypothesis example psychology

Even if a brain region did in fact execute the same level of local neuronal cortical processing for real sentences and Jabberwocky sentences with respect to building inferences of recursive hierarchical structure, the lexico-semantic differences would naturally interfere here, given the close regulation of semantics by syntax.

In other words, why would we expect to find a “pure” syntactic response profile when syntax is itself anything but pure? It is in the game of regulating semantic structure via a complex interface of 'morphophonological' transformations .

In addition, while one of the advantages of fMRI is its spatial resolution, the authors keep to large macroanatomical areas (i.e. the entire anterior temporal surface, the entire posterior temporal surface, etc.), which may be neurobiologically implausible in terms of making spatiotemporal claims of cortical computation and the possibility of distinct response profiles that might, nevertheless, bias much more heavily towards combinatorial semantic and/or syntactic information.

The authors also cite Fedorenko’s intracranial study (using electrodes recorded directly from the surface of the brain in epilepsy patients), and Nelson and colleague’s intracranial study, using this to motivate their analysis of nodal structure-building. They do not cite more recent intracranial work failing to replicate much of Nelson and colleagues’ major effects of structure (including discussion of why this might be the case).

The authors’ finding that “no language region appears to be a hub for abstract (i.e., content-independent) combinatorics” speaks to the idea of unbounded hierarchical structure-generation being uniquely tied to its elements of selection, as per many models of minimalist syntax, as opposed to models that claim, for instance, that the human brain has a domain-general structure-building capacity that applies over distinct representational domains (e.g., language, mathematics, music, etc).

Overall, it may not be the case, as some have claimed (e.g., Dehaene, Fitch, Bornkessel-Schlesewsky) that there are dedicated neural circuits for abstract combinatorics. Instead, human language might be built off a constellation of domain-general neural codes but with unique integration of these codes alongside domain-specific stored representations (as proposed in the recent ROSE model for the brain’s neurocomputational architecture for language, in which lexical units are intrinsically maintained in their base code throughout higher-order syntactic operations), partly accounting for the kind of “difference of degree but not kind” perspective unveiled by Shain and colleagues. Still, there may be types of neural signatures that fall outside of the poor temporal resolution afforded by fMRI that can detect more rapid shifts between distinct levels of linguistic structure.

Elliot Murphy Ph.D.

Elliot Murphy, Ph.D. , is the author of The Oscillatory Nature of Language.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

May 2024 magazine cover

At any moment, someone’s aggravating behavior or our own bad luck can set us off on an emotional spiral that threatens to derail our entire day. Here’s how we can face our triggers with less reactivity so that we can get on with our lives.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Effects of Test Anxiety on Self-Testing and Learning Performance

  • Published: 07 June 2024
  • Volume 36 , article number  59 , ( 2024 )

Cite this article

research hypothesis example psychology

  • Shaohang Liu   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1572-5245 1 , 2 ,
  • Wenbo Zhao 3 ,
  • David R. Shanks 4 ,
  • Xiao Hu 2 , 5 ,
  • Liang Luo 1 , 2 &
  • Chunliang Yang 1 , 2  

Practice testing (i.e., practice retrieval) has been established as an effective learning strategy. Uncovering potential factors influencing self-testing usage is a prerequisite to promote its practical use. The present study reports five experiments exploring whether test anxiety (TA) and test stake (1) affect self-testing usage (Experiments 1–5) and (2) influence learning performance through their negative effects on self-testing usage (Experiments 1, 4, and 5). Experiment 1 analyzed data from 459 high school students collected via a survey and found both that TA negatively predicted students’ daily use of self-testing and that self-testing usage mediated the negative association between TA and academic performance. The negative association between TA and self-testing usage was further replicated in a laboratory experiment (Experiment 2). Another quasi-experiment (Experiment 3) showed that students were less likely to test themselves when preparing for a high-stake than a low-stake exam. Experiment 4 replicated this finding and additionally demonstrated that a high-stake test led to poorer learning via its negative influence on self-testing usage. Experiment 5 demonstrated that a high-stake test provoked high state anxiety, which then induced avoidance of self-testing and ultimately impaired learning. Overall, these findings demonstrate a negative effect of TA on self-testing usage, in turn leading to poor learning. Practical implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

research hypothesis example psychology

Abu-Bader, S., & Jones, T. V. (2021). Statistical mediation analysis using the Sobel test and Hayes SPSS Process Macro. International Journal of Quantitative and Qualitative Research Methods , Retrieved March 25, 2024 from https://ssrn.com/abstract=3799204

Adesola, S. A., & Li, Y. (2018). The relationship between self-regulation, self-efficacy, test anxiety and motivation. International Journal of Information and Education Technology, 8 , 759–763. https://doi.org/10.18178/ijiet.2018.8.10.1135

Article   Google Scholar  

Agarwal, P. K., D’Antonio, L., Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Classroom-based programs of retrieval practice reduce middle school and high school students’ test anxiety. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3 , 131–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.07.002

Badali, S., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2022). Do students effectively regulate their use of self-testing as a function of item difficulty? Educational Psychology Review, 34 , 1651–1677. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-022-09665-6

Bartoszewski, B. L., & Gurung, R. A. R. (2015). Comparing the relationship of learning techniques and exam score. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology , 1, 219–228. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000036

Bernacki, M. L., Vosicka, L., & Utz, J. C. (2020). Can a brief, digital skill training intervention help undergraduates “learn to learn” and improve their STEM achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 112 , 765–781. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000405

Biwer, F., Egbrink, M. G., Aalten, P., & de Bruin, A. B. (2020). Fostering effective learning strategies in higher education—a mixed-methods study. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition , 9, 186–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.03.004

Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64 , 417–444. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823

Blackwell, L. (2002). Psychological mediators of student achievement during the transition to junior high school: The role of implicit theories. Columbia University . Retrieved October 21, 2023 from https://www.proquest.com/docview/304792082?pq-origsite=gscholar&fromopenview=true

Bottiroli, S., Dunlosky, J., Guerini, K., Cavallini, E., & Hertzog, C. (2010). Does task affordance moderate age-related deficits in strategy production? Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 17 , 591–602. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2010.481356

Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1994). Collective self-esteem consequences of outgroup derogation when a valued social identity is on trial. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24 , 641–657. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420240603

Carpenter, S. K. (2009). Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: The benefits of elaborative retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35 , 1563–1569. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017021

Carpenter, S. K. (2012). Testing enhances the transfer of learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21 , 279–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412452728

Cassady, J. C. (2004). The influence of cognitive test anxiety across the learning–testing cycle. Learning and Instruction, 14 , 569–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.09.002

Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27 , 270–295. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2001.1094

Cipra, C., & Müller-Hilke, B. (2019). Testing anxiety in undergraduate medical students and its correlation with different learning approaches. PLOS ONE , 14 . https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210130

Clark, D. A., & Svinicki, M. (2015). The effect of retrieval on post-task enjoyment of studying. Educational Psychology Review, 27 , 51–67. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9272-4

Costa, A., & Faria, L. (2018). Implicit theories of intelligence and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Frontiers in Psychology , 9 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00829

Culler, R. E., & Holahan, C. J. (1980). Test anxiety and academic performance: The effects of study-related behaviors. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72 , 16–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.72.1.16

Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14 , 4–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266

Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95 , 256–273. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256

Endler, N. S., & Kocovski, N. L. (2001). State and trait anxiety revisited. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 15 , 231–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6185(01)00060-3

Ergene, T. (2003). Effective interventions on test anxiety reduction. School Psychology International, 24 , 313–328. https://doi.org/10.1177/01430343030243004

Ewell, S. N., Josefson, C. C., & Ballen, C. J. (2022). Why did students report lower test anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic? Journal of Microbiology & Biology Education , 23 . https://doi.org/10.1128/jmbe.00282-21

Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition & Emotion, 6 , 409–434. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208409696

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7 , 336–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

Fairchild, A. J., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). A general model for testing mediation and moderation effects. Prevention Science, 10 , 87–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-008-0109-6

Fan, T., Yang, C., & Luo, L. (2023). Improving the use of retrieval practice for both easy and difficult materials: The effect of an instructional intervention. In preparation for publication.

Geller, J., Toftness, A. R., Armstrong, P. I., Carpenter, S. K., Manz, C. L., Coffman, C. R., & Lamm, M. H. (2018). Study strategies and beliefs about learning as a function of academic achievement and achievement goals. Memory, 26 , 683–690. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1397175

Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2012). Study strategies of college students: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19 , 126–134. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0181-y

Heitmann, S., Grund, A., Berthold, K., Fries, S., & Roelle, J. (2018). Testing is more desirable when it is adaptive and still desirable when compared to note-taking. Frontiers in Psychology , 9 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02596

Hinze, S. R., & Rapp, D. N. (2014). Retrieval (sometimes) enhances learning: Performance pressure reduces the benefits of retrieval practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28 , 597–606. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3032

Hui, L., de Bruin, A. B. H., Donkers, J., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2021). Does individual performance feedback increase the use of retrieval practice? Educational Psychology Review, 33 , 1835–1857. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-021-09604-x

Hunsley, J. (1985). Test anxiety, academic performance, and cognitive appraisals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77 , 678–682. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.77.6.678

JASP Team. (2023). JASP (Version 0.17.1) [Computer software].

Johnson, M. L., & Sinatra, G. M. (2013). Use of task-value instructional inductions for facilitating engagement and conceptual change. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38 , 51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.003

Karatas, H., Alci, B., & Aydin, H. (2013). Correlation among high school senior students’ test anxiety, academic performance and points of university entrance exam. Educational Research and Reviews, 8 , 919–926. https://doi.org/10.5897/ERR2013.1462

Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331 , 772–775. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199327

Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice promotes short-term retention, but equally spaced retrieval enhances long-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 , 704–719. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.704

Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2009). Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own? Memory, 17 , 471–479. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802647009

Karpicke, J. D. (2017). Retrieval-based learning: A decade of progress. In Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference , 487–514. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.21055-9

Khanna, M. M. (2015). Ungraded pop quizzes: Test-enhanced learning without all the anxiety. Teaching of Psychology, 42 , 174–178. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628315573144

Kirk-Johnson, A., Galla, B. M., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2019). Perceiving effort as poor learning: The misinterpreted-effort hypothesis of how experienced effort and perceived learning relate to study strategy choice. Cognitive Psychology, 115 , 101237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.101237

Kirkland, K., & Hollandsworth, J. G. (1980). Effective test taking: Skills-acquisition versus anxiety-reduction techniques. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48 , 431–439. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.48.4.431

Koriat, A. (2008). Subjective confidence in one’s answers: The consensuality principle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 , 945–959. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.945

Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14 , 219–224. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194055

Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory, 17 , 493–501. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210902832915

Li, B., Zhao, W., Shi, A., Zhong, Y., Hu, X., Liu, M., Luo, L., & Yang, C. (2023). Does the reactivity effect of judgments of learning transfer to learning of new information? Memory, 3 (1), 918–930. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2208792

Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20 , 975–978. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3.975

Liu, S., Luo, L., & Yang, C. (2023). Test anxiety negatively predicts metacognitive awareness of test-enhanced learning and self-testing usage. In preparation for publication.

Ma, X., Li, T., Jia, R., & Wei, J. (2022). The forward testing effect in spatial route learning. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54 , 1433. https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2022.01433

McAndrew, M., Morrow, C. S., Atiyeh, L., & Pierre, G. C. (2016). Dental student study strategies: Are self-testing and scheduling related to academic performance? Journal of Dental Education, 80 (5), 542–552. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.0022-0337.2016.80.5.tb06114.x

McCabe, J. A., & Lummis, S. N. (2018). Why and how do undergraduates study in groups? Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 4 , 27–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000099

McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2020). Training learning strategies to promote self-regulation and transfer: The knowledge, belief, commitment, and planning framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15 , 1363–1381. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620920723

Montoya, A. K. (2019). Moderation analysis in two-instance repeated measures designs: Probing methods and multiple moderator models. Behavior Research Methods. 51 , 61-82. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1088-6

Morris, L. W., & Fulmer, R. S. (1976). Test anxiety (worry and emotionality) changes during academic testing as a function of feedback and test importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68 , 817–824. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.68.6.817

Myers, S. J., Davis, S. D., & Chan, J. C. K. (2021). Does expressive writing or an instructional intervention reduce the impacts of test anxiety in a college classroom? Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 6 , 44. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-021-00309-x

Park, D., Gunderson, E. A., Tsukayama, E., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). Young children’s motivational frameworks and math achievement: Relation to teacher-reported instructional practices, but not teacher theory of intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108 , 300–313. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000064

Pastötter, B., & Bäuml, K. H. T. (2014). Retrieval practice enhances new learning: The forward effect of testing. Frontiers in Psychology , 5 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00286

Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162 , 8–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017

Putwain, D., & Daly, A. L. (2014). Test anxiety prevalence and gender differences in a sample of English secondary school students. Educational Studies, 40 , 554–570. https://doi.org/10.1080/03055698.2014.953914

Putwain, D. W., & Symes, W. (2011). Achievement goals as mediators of the relationship between competence beliefs and test anxiety. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82 , 207–224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02021.x

Putwain, D. W., & von der Embse, N. P. (2021). Cognitive–behavioral intervention for test anxiety in adolescent students: Do benefits extend to school-related wellbeing and clinical anxiety. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 34 , 22–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2020.1800656

Putwain, D. W., Connors, L., Symes, W., & Douglas-Osborn, E. (2012). Is academic buoyancy anything more than adaptive coping? Anxiety, Stress, & Coping , 25, 349–358. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2011.582459

Putwain, D. W., Gallard, D., Beaumont, J., Loderer, K., & von der Embse, N. P. (2021). Does test anxiety predispose poor school-related wellbeing and enhanced risk of emotional disorders? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 45 , 1150–1162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-021-10211-x

Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Optimizing schedules of retrieval practice for durable and efficient learning: How much is enough? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 140, 283–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023956

Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2012). When is practice testing most effective for improving the durability and efficiency of student learning? Educational Psychology Review, 24 , 419–435. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9203-1

Rea, S. D., Wang, L., Muenks, K., & Yan, V. X. (2022). Students can (mostly) recognize effective learning, so why do they not do it? Journal of Intelligence, 10 , 127. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040127

Rivers, M. L. (2021). Metacognition about practice testing: A review of learners’ beliefs, monitoring, and control of test-enhanced learning. Educational Psychology Review, 33 , 823–862. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09578-2

Rivers, M. L., Dunlosky, J., & McLeod, M. (2022). What constrains people’s ability to learn about the testing effect through task experience? Memory, 30 , 1387–1404. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2022.2120204

Robson, D. A., Johnstone, S. J., Putwain, D. W., & Howard, S. (2023). Test anxiety in primary school children: A 20-year systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 98 , 39–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2023.02.003

Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17 , 249–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x

Rohrer, D., Taylor, K., & Sholar, B. (2010). Tests enhance the transfer of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36 , 233–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017678

Roick, J., & Ringeisen, T. (2017). Self-efficacy, test anxiety, and academic success: A longitudinal validation. International Journal of Educational Research, 83 , 84–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2016.12.006

Roos, A.-L., Goetz, T., Voracek, M., Krannich, M., Bieg, M., Jarrell, A., & Pekrun, R. (2021). Test anxiety and physiological arousal: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 33 , 579–618. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09543-z

Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140 , 1432–1463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037559

Sarason, I. G., & Stoops, R. (1978). Test anxiety and the passage of time. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 , 102–109. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.46.1.102

Segool, N. K., Carlson, J. S., Goforth, A. N., von der Embse, N., & Barterian, J. A. (2013). Heightened test anxiety among young children: Elementary school students’ anxious responses to high-stakes testing. Psychology in the Schools, 50 , 489–499. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21689

Shanks, D. R., Don, H. J., Boustani, S., & Yang, C. (2023). Test-enhanced learning. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology . https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.908

Shi, A., Xu, C., Zhao, W., Shanks, D. R., Hu, X., Luo, L., & Yang, C. (2022). Judgments of learning reactively facilitate visual memory by enhancing learning engagement. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30 , 676–687. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02174-1

Soares, D., & Woods, K. (2020). An international systematic literature review of test anxiety interventions 2011–2018. Pastoral Care in Education, 38 , 311–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643944.2020.1725909

Sotola, L. K., & Crede, M. (2021). Regarding class quizzes: A meta-analytic synthesis of studies on the relationship between frequent low-stakes testing and class performance. Educational Psychology Review, 33 , 407–426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09563-9

Spielberger, C. D. (1980). Test anxiety inventory: Preliminary professional manual . Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar  

Spielberger, C. D., & Vagg, P. R. (1995). Test anxiety: A transactional process model. In C. D. Spielberger & P. R. Vagg (Eds.), Test anxiety: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 3–14). Taylor & Francis.

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Steinmayr, R., Crede, J., McElvany, N., & Wirthwein, L. (2016). Subjective well-being, test anxiety, academic achievement: Testing for reciprocal effects. Frontiers in Psychology , 6 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01994

Stewart, D., Panus, P., Hagemeier, N., Thigpen, J., & Brooks, L. (2014). Pharmacy student self-testing as a predictor of examination performance. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 78 , 32. https://doi.org/10.5688/ajpe78232

Stowell, J. R., & Bennett, D. (2010). Effects of online testing on student exam performance and test anxiety. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 42 , 161–171. https://doi.org/10.2190/EC.42.2.b

Sun, Y., Shi, A., Zhao, W., Yang, Y., Li, B., Hu, X., Shanks, D. R., Yang, C., & Luo, L. (2022). Long-lasting effects of an instructional intervention on interleaving preference in inductive learning and transfer. Educational Psychology Review, 34 , 1679–1707. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-022-09666-5

Symes, W., & Putwain, D. W. (2020). The four WS of test anxiety. Psychologica, 63 , 31–52. https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_63-2_2

Szafranski, D. D., Barrera, T. L., & Norton, P. J. (2012). Test anxiety inventory: 30 years later. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 25 , 667–677. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2012.663490

Szpunar, K. K., Khan, N. Y., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Interpolated memory tests reduce mind wandering and improve learning of online lectures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 , 6313–6317. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1221764110

Theobald, M., Breitwieser, J., & Brod, G. (2022). Test anxiety does not predict exam performance when knowledge is controlled for: Strong evidence against the interference hypothesis of test anxiety. Psychological Science, 33 , 2073–2083. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221119391

Thomas, C. L., Cassady, J. C., & Finch, W. H. (2018). Identifying severity standards on the cognitive test anxiety scale: Cut score determination using latent class and cluster analysis. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 36 , 492–508. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282916686004

Tse, C.-S., Chan, M. H.-M., Tse, W.-S., & Wong, S. W.-H. (2019). Can the testing effect for general knowledge facts be influenced by distraction due to divided attention or experimentally induced anxious mood? Frontiers in Psychology , 10 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00969

Tse, C.-S., & Pu, X. (2012). The effectiveness of test-enhanced learning depends on trait test anxiety and working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18 , 253–264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029190

Toppino, T. C., LaVan, M. H., & Iaconelli, R. T. (2018). Metacognitive control in self-regulated learning: Conditions affecting the choice of restudying versus retrieval practice. Memory & Cognition , 46, 1164-1177. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0828-2

Tullis, J. G., Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A. S. (2013). Metacognition of the testing effect: Guiding learners to predict the benefits of retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 41 , 429–442. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0274-5

Tullis, J. G., Fiechter, J. L., & Benjamin, A. S. (2018). The efficacy of learners’ testing choices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44 , 540–552. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000473

Vaughn, K. E., & Kornell, N. (2019). How to activate students’ natural desire to test themselves. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 4 , 35. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-019-0187-y

von der Embse, N., Jester, D., Roy, D., & Post, J. (2018). Test anxiety effects, predictors, and correlates: A 30-year meta-analytic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 227 , 483–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.048

Weissgerber, S. C., & Rummer, R. (2023). More accurate than assumed: Learners’ metacognitive beliefs about the effectiveness of retrieval practice. Learning and Instruction, 83 , 101679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2022.101679

Wenzel, K., & Reinhard, M.-A. (2021). Does the end justify the means? Learning tests lead to more negative evaluations and to more stress experiences. Learning and Motivation, 73 , 101706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2020.101706

Wissman, K. T., & Rawson, K. A. (2016). How do students implement collaborative testing in real-world contexts? Memory, 24 , 223–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.999792

Yan, V. X., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2016). On the difficulty of mending metacognitive illusions: A priori theories, fluency effects, and misattributions of the interleaving benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145 , 918–933. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000177

Yang, C., Potts, R., & Shanks, D. R. (2018). Enhancing learning and retrieval of new information: A review of the forward testing effect. npj Science of Learning, 3 , 8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-018-0024-y

Yang, C., Sun, B., Potts, R., Yu, R., Luo, L., & Shanks, D. R. (2020). Do working memory capacity and test anxiety modulate the beneficial effects of testing on new learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 26 , 724–738. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000278

Yang, C., Luo, L., Vadillo, M. A., Yu, R., & Shanks, D. R. (2021). Testing (quizzing) boosts classroom learning: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 147 , 399–435. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000309

Yang, C., Li, J., Zhao, W., Luo, L., & Shanks, D. (2023). Do practice tests (quizzes) reduce or provoke test anxiety? A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 35 , 87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09801-wP

Yeager, D. S., Romero, C., Paunesku, D., Hulleman, C. S., Schneider, B., Hinojosa, C., Lee, H. Y., O’Brien, J., Flint, K., Roberts, A., Trott, J., Greene, D., Walton, G. M., & Dweck, C. S. (2016). Using design thinking to improve psychological interventions: The case of the growth mindset during the transition to high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108 , 374–391. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000098

Yue, X. (1996). Test anxiety and self-efficacy: Levels and relationship among secondary school students in Hong Kong. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient .

Zhao, W., Yin, Y., Hu, X., Shanks, D. R., Yang, C., & Luo, L. (2023). Memory for inter-item relations is reactively disrupted by metamemory judgments. Metacognition and Learning, 18 , 549–566. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-023-09340-3

Weissgerber, S. C., Reinhard, M. A., & Schindler, S. (2018). Learning the hard way: Need for Cognition influences attitudes toward and self-reported use of desirable difficulties. Educational Psychology , 38, 176–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2017.1387644

Download references

This research was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of China (32000742; 32171045; 32200841), the Research Program Funds of the Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment toward Basic Education Quality at Beijing Normal University (2021–01-132-BZK01), the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ES/S014616/1), and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2022NTSS36).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Institute of Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Haidian District, 19 Xinjiekouwai Street, Beijing, 100875, China

Shaohang Liu, Liang Luo & Chunliang Yang

Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Shaohang Liu, Xiao Hu, Liang Luo & Chunliang Yang

Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment for Basic Education Quality, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK

David R. Shanks

Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chunliang Yang .

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author Note

All data and experimental stimuli have been made publicly available via the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/3czqr/ .

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Liu, S., Zhao, W., Shanks, D.R. et al. Effects of Test Anxiety on Self-Testing and Learning Performance. Educ Psychol Rev 36 , 59 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09882-1

Download citation

Accepted : 29 March 2024

Published : 07 June 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09882-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Test anxiety
  • Self-testing
  • Learning performance
  • Test-enhanced learning
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Logo for Kwantlen Polytechnic University

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

Overview of the Scientific Method

10 Developing a Hypothesis

Learning objectives.

  • Distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis.
  • Discover how theories are used to generate hypotheses and how the results of studies can be used to further inform theories.
  • Understand the characteristics of a good hypothesis.

Theories and Hypotheses

Before describing how to develop a hypothesis, it is important to distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis. A  theory  is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes, functions, or organizing principles that have not been observed directly. Consider, for example, Zajonc’s theory of social facilitation and social inhibition (1965) [1] . He proposed that being watched by others while performing a task creates a general state of physiological arousal, which increases the likelihood of the dominant (most likely) response. So for highly practiced tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make correct responses, but for relatively unpracticed tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make incorrect responses. Notice that this theory—which has come to be called drive theory—provides an explanation of both social facilitation and social inhibition that goes beyond the phenomena themselves by including concepts such as “arousal” and “dominant response,” along with processes such as the effect of arousal on the dominant response.

Outside of science, referring to an idea as a theory often implies that it is untested—perhaps no more than a wild guess. In science, however, the term theory has no such implication. A theory is simply an explanation or interpretation of a set of phenomena. It can be untested, but it can also be extensively tested, well supported, and accepted as an accurate description of the world by the scientific community. The theory of evolution by natural selection, for example, is a theory because it is an explanation of the diversity of life on earth—not because it is untested or unsupported by scientific research. On the contrary, the evidence for this theory is overwhelmingly positive and nearly all scientists accept its basic assumptions as accurate. Similarly, the “germ theory” of disease is a theory because it is an explanation of the origin of various diseases, not because there is any doubt that many diseases are caused by microorganisms that infect the body.

A  hypothesis , on the other hand, is a specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate. It is an explanation that relies on just a few key concepts. Hypotheses are often specific predictions about what will happen in a particular study. They are developed by considering existing evidence and using reasoning to infer what will happen in the specific context of interest. Hypotheses are often but not always derived from theories. So a hypothesis is often a prediction based on a theory but some hypotheses are a-theoretical and only after a set of observations have been made, is a theory developed. This is because theories are broad in nature and they explain larger bodies of data. So if our research question is really original then we may need to collect some data and make some observations before we can develop a broader theory.

Theories and hypotheses always have this  if-then  relationship. “ If   drive theory is correct,  then  cockroaches should run through a straight runway faster, and a branching runway more slowly, when other cockroaches are present.” Although hypotheses are usually expressed as statements, they can always be rephrased as questions. “Do cockroaches run through a straight runway faster when other cockroaches are present?” Thus deriving hypotheses from theories is an excellent way of generating interesting research questions.

But how do researchers derive hypotheses from theories? One way is to generate a research question using the techniques discussed in this chapter  and then ask whether any theory implies an answer to that question. For example, you might wonder whether expressive writing about positive experiences improves health as much as expressive writing about traumatic experiences. Although this  question  is an interesting one  on its own, you might then ask whether the habituation theory—the idea that expressive writing causes people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings—implies an answer. In this case, it seems clear that if the habituation theory is correct, then expressive writing about positive experiences should not be effective because it would not cause people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings. A second way to derive hypotheses from theories is to focus on some component of the theory that has not yet been directly observed. For example, a researcher could focus on the process of habituation—perhaps hypothesizing that people should show fewer signs of emotional distress with each new writing session.

Among the very best hypotheses are those that distinguish between competing theories. For example, Norbert Schwarz and his colleagues considered two theories of how people make judgments about themselves, such as how assertive they are (Schwarz et al., 1991) [2] . Both theories held that such judgments are based on relevant examples that people bring to mind. However, one theory was that people base their judgments on the  number  of examples they bring to mind and the other was that people base their judgments on how  easily  they bring those examples to mind. To test these theories, the researchers asked people to recall either six times when they were assertive (which is easy for most people) or 12 times (which is difficult for most people). Then they asked them to judge their own assertiveness. Note that the number-of-examples theory implies that people who recalled 12 examples should judge themselves to be more assertive because they recalled more examples, but the ease-of-examples theory implies that participants who recalled six examples should judge themselves as more assertive because recalling the examples was easier. Thus the two theories made opposite predictions so that only one of the predictions could be confirmed. The surprising result was that participants who recalled fewer examples judged themselves to be more assertive—providing particularly convincing evidence in favor of the ease-of-retrieval theory over the number-of-examples theory.

Theory Testing

The primary way that scientific researchers use theories is sometimes called the hypothetico-deductive method  (although this term is much more likely to be used by philosophers of science than by scientists themselves). Researchers begin with a set of phenomena and either construct a theory to explain or interpret them or choose an existing theory to work with. They then make a prediction about some new phenomenon that should be observed if the theory is correct. Again, this prediction is called a hypothesis. The researchers then conduct an empirical study to test the hypothesis. Finally, they reevaluate the theory in light of the new results and revise it if necessary. This process is usually conceptualized as a cycle because the researchers can then derive a new hypothesis from the revised theory, conduct a new empirical study to test the hypothesis, and so on. As  Figure 2.3  shows, this approach meshes nicely with the model of scientific research in psychology presented earlier in the textbook—creating a more detailed model of “theoretically motivated” or “theory-driven” research.

research hypothesis example psychology

As an example, let us consider Zajonc’s research on social facilitation and inhibition. He started with a somewhat contradictory pattern of results from the research literature. He then constructed his drive theory, according to which being watched by others while performing a task causes physiological arousal, which increases an organism’s tendency to make the dominant response. This theory predicts social facilitation for well-learned tasks and social inhibition for poorly learned tasks. He now had a theory that organized previous results in a meaningful way—but he still needed to test it. He hypothesized that if his theory was correct, he should observe that the presence of others improves performance in a simple laboratory task but inhibits performance in a difficult version of the very same laboratory task. To test this hypothesis, one of the studies he conducted used cockroaches as subjects (Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman, 1969) [3] . The cockroaches ran either down a straight runway (an easy task for a cockroach) or through a cross-shaped maze (a difficult task for a cockroach) to escape into a dark chamber when a light was shined on them. They did this either while alone or in the presence of other cockroaches in clear plastic “audience boxes.” Zajonc found that cockroaches in the straight runway reached their goal more quickly in the presence of other cockroaches, but cockroaches in the cross-shaped maze reached their goal more slowly when they were in the presence of other cockroaches. Thus he confirmed his hypothesis and provided support for his drive theory. (Zajonc also showed that drive theory existed in humans [Zajonc & Sales, 1966] [4] in many other studies afterward).

Incorporating Theory into Your Research

When you write your research report or plan your presentation, be aware that there are two basic ways that researchers usually include theory. The first is to raise a research question, answer that question by conducting a new study, and then offer one or more theories (usually more) to explain or interpret the results. This format works well for applied research questions and for research questions that existing theories do not address. The second way is to describe one or more existing theories, derive a hypothesis from one of those theories, test the hypothesis in a new study, and finally reevaluate the theory. This format works well when there is an existing theory that addresses the research question—especially if the resulting hypothesis is surprising or conflicts with a hypothesis derived from a different theory.

To use theories in your research will not only give you guidance in coming up with experiment ideas and possible projects, but it lends legitimacy to your work. Psychologists have been interested in a variety of human behaviors and have developed many theories along the way. Using established theories will help you break new ground as a researcher, not limit you from developing your own ideas.

Characteristics of a Good Hypothesis

There are three general characteristics of a good hypothesis. First, a good hypothesis must be testable and falsifiable . We must be able to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and if you’ll recall Popper’s falsifiability criterion, it must be possible to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false. Second, a good hypothesis must be logical. As described above, hypotheses are more than just a random guess. Hypotheses should be informed by previous theories or observations and logical reasoning. Typically, we begin with a broad and general theory and use  deductive reasoning to generate a more specific hypothesis to test based on that theory. Occasionally, however, when there is no theory to inform our hypothesis, we use  inductive reasoning  which involves using specific observations or research findings to form a more general hypothesis. Finally, the hypothesis should be positive. That is, the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a relationship or effect, rather than a statement that a relationship or effect does not exist. As scientists, we don’t set out to show that relationships do not exist or that effects do not occur so our hypotheses should not be worded in a way to suggest that an effect or relationship does not exist. The nature of science is to assume that something does not exist and then seek to find evidence to prove this wrong, to show that it really does exist. That may seem backward to you but that is the nature of the scientific method. The underlying reason for this is beyond the scope of this chapter but it has to do with statistical theory.

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation.  Science, 149 , 269–274 ↵
  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 195–202. ↵
  • Zajonc, R. B., Heingartner, A., & Herman, E. M. (1969). Social enhancement and impairment of performance in the cockroach.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13 , 83–92. ↵
  • Zajonc, R.B. & Sales, S.M. (1966). Social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 , 160-168. ↵

A coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena.

A specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate.

A cyclical process of theory development, starting with an observed phenomenon, then developing or using a theory to make a specific prediction of what should happen if that theory is correct, testing that prediction, refining the theory in light of the findings, and using that refined theory to develop new hypotheses, and so on.

The ability to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and the possibility to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false.

Research Methods in Psychology Copyright © 2019 by Rajiv S. Jhangiani, I-Chant A. Chiang, Carrie Cuttler, & Dana C. Leighton is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

IMAGES

  1. 13 Different Types of Hypothesis (2024)

    research hypothesis example psychology

  2. 100+ Psychology Hypothesis Statement Examples, How to Write, Tips

    research hypothesis example psychology

  3. how to write a research hypothesis psychology

    research hypothesis example psychology

  4. What is a Hypothesis

    research hypothesis example psychology

  5. How To Write A Hypothesis For A Lab Report

    research hypothesis example psychology

  6. glossary initiativeblog.com

    research hypothesis example psychology

VIDEO

  1. Two-Sample Hypothesis Testing: Dependent Sample

  2. Concept of Hypothesis in Hindi || Research Hypothesis || #ugcnetphysicaleducation #ntaugcnet

  3. What is Hypothesis? Example of Hypothesis [#shorts] [#statistics

  4. Research Methods Q2: Hypothesis Writing

  5. Hypothesis Testing in Psychological Research

  6. Research Hypothesis and its Types with examples /urdu/hindi

COMMENTS

  1. Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

    Examples. A research hypothesis, in its plural form "hypotheses," is a specific, testable prediction about the anticipated results of a study, established at its outset. It is a key component of the scientific method. Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding.

  2. Hypothesis: Definition, Examples, and Types

    This article explores how a hypothesis is used in psychology research, how to write a good hypothesis, and the different types of hypotheses you might use. The Hypothesis in the Scientific Method . ... For example, prior research has shown that stress can impact the immune system. So a researcher might hypothesize: "People with high-stress ...

  3. 6 Hypothesis Examples in Psychology

    Alternative Hypothesis: Eating an apple daily reduces the chances of visiting the doctor. Null Hypothesis: Eating an apple daily does not impact the frequency of visiting the doctor. Example 2. Research Problem: What is the impact of spending a lot of time on mobiles on the attention span of teenagers.

  4. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis

    Developing a hypothesis (with example) Step 1. Ask a question. Writing a hypothesis begins with a research question that you want to answer. The question should be focused, specific, and researchable within the constraints of your project. Example: Research question.

  5. Aims and Hypotheses

    The theory attempting to explain an observation will help to inform hypotheses - predictions of an investigation's outcome that make specific reference to the independent variables (IVs) manipulated and dependent variables (DVs) measured by the researchers. There are two types of hypothesis: H1 - The Research Hypothesis.

  6. Aims and Hypotheses: Explanation, Examples, Type & Psychology

    There are two types of hypotheses: null and alternative hypotheses. A null hypothesis states that the independent variable does not influence the dependent variable. The null hypothesis states that changes/ manipulating the IV will not affect the DV. Research scenario: Investigation of how test results affect sleep.

  7. Research questions and hypotheses.

    In this chapter, we examine the principal types of research questions or problems, give examples of each, and review the design expectations that each type of question establishes. ... Research questions and hypotheses. In J. Meltzoff & H. Cooper, Critical thinking about research: Psychology and related fields (2nd ed., pp. 23-41). American ...

  8. Developing a Hypothesis

    Figure 2.2 Hypothetico-Deductive Method Combined With the General Model of Scientific Research in Psychology Together they form a model of theoretically motivated research. As an example, let us consider Zajonc's research on social facilitation and inhibition. ... the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a ...

  9. Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

    The research guess is much transferred to more the alternative hypothesis. A research hypothesis, in her plural form "hypotheses," shall a customizable, testable project about an anticipated results of a study, established at its ausgang. ... Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples. By. Saint Mcleod, PhD. Updated on. December 69 ...

  10. PDF When carefully considered, almost any research hypothesis or Research

    Research Hypotheses and Multiple Regression, cont. 3. The differential performance of a specific model across two or more groups (populations, treatments, etc.) • Whether the model produces equivalent R2 for the groups • Allows us to look for much more than "mean differences" • Important for population generalizability questions

  11. PDF Task 4

    Task 1: Without knowing much about how to write a hypothesis in psychology, try and write a hypothesis for this research aim: investigating the power of uniforms in obedience. Here is an example of hypothesis for a different research aim. Look how the Aim has been turned into something a researcher could actually test.... Research aim ...

  12. Hypothesis

    Hypothesis. In scientific research, a hypothesis is a statement about a predicted relationship between variables. A good research hypothesis can be formulated as an "if-then" statement: If a child is exposed to the music of Mozart, then that child's intelligence will increase. If students learn a math lesson by interacting with a computer ...

  13. Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

    The research hypothesis is often consulted until as the alternative hypothesis. Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples - What is and How to Write a Good Hypothesis in Research? A research hypothesis, includes its plural form "hypotheses," is a specialist, testable prediction about the anticipated earnings of one study, established ...

  14. Research Hypothesis: Definition, Types, Examples and Quick Tips

    3. Simple hypothesis. A simple hypothesis is a statement made to reflect the relation between exactly two variables. One independent and one dependent. Consider the example, "Smoking is a prominent cause of lung cancer." The dependent variable, lung cancer, is dependent on the independent variable, smoking. 4.

  15. The Three Most Common Types of Hypotheses

    In this post, I'll briefly describe the three most common kinds of hypotheses that expand upon simple main effects - at least, the most common ones I've seen in my research career in psychology - as well as providing some resources to help you learn about how to test these hypotheses using statistics. Incremental Validity.

  16. Hypotheses

    Writing good hypotheses in IB Psychology IAs is something many students find challenging. After moderating another 175+ IA's this year I could see some common errors students were making. This post hopes to give a clear explanation with examples to help with this tricky task. Null and Alternative Hypotheses Null Hypothesis (H0)

  17. Research Question and Hypothesis

    Psychology Research Question and Hypothesis Statement Examples. Psychology is the scientific study of human behavior and mental processes. Psychology research questions delve into various aspects of human behavior, cognition, emotion, and more. These questions are designed to gain a deeper understanding of psychological phenomena.

  18. APA Dictionary of Psychology

    A trusted reference in the field of psychology, offering more than 25,000 clear and authoritative entries. ... research hypothesis. Share button. Updated on 04/19/2018. a statement describing the investigator's expectation about the pattern of data that may result from a given study. By stating specific expectations before the data are ...

  19. What Is Chi Square Test & How To Calculate Formula Equation

    Formula Calculation. Calculate the chi-square statistic (χ2) by completing the following steps: Calculate the expected frequencies and the observed frequencies. For each observed number in the table, subtract the corresponding expected number (O — E). Square the difference (O —E)². Sum all the values for (O - E)² / E.

  20. Hypothesis Testing

    Hypothesis Testing. Hypothesis testing is an important feature of science, as this is how theories are developed and modified. A good theory should generate testable predictions (hypotheses), and if research fails to support the hypotheses, then this suggests that the theory needs to be modified in some way.

  21. Building up Hypotheses in Clinical Psychology and Neuroscience

    Vol. 8 (2017), n. 1, pp. 110-117. Buil ding up Hypoth eses in Clinic al Psycho logy and. Neur osci ence: Simi lari ties and D iffe rence s. Sofia Adelaide Osimo (α) & Raffaella Ida Rumiati (α ...

  22. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis

    Step 5: Phrase your hypothesis in three ways. To identify the variables, you can write a simple prediction in if … then form. The first part of the sentence states the independent variable and the second part states the dependent variable. If a first-year student starts attending more lectures, then their exam scores will improve.

  23. The double empathy problem: A derivation chain analysis and cautionary

    Work on the "double empathy problem" (DEP) is rapidly growing in academic and applied settings (e.g., clinical practice). It is most popular in research on conditions, like autism, which are characterized by social cognitive difficulties. Drawing from this literature, we propose that, while research on the DEP has the potential to improve understanding of both typical and atypical social ...

  24. How the Brain Processes Different Components of Language

    The research is an excellent example of innovative neuroscience methods being paired with thoughtful and critical linguistic hypotheses to deliver novel insights on the question: "Which parts of ...

  25. Effects of Test Anxiety on Self-Testing and Learning Performance

    Practice testing (i.e., practice retrieval) has been established as an effective learning strategy. Uncovering potential factors influencing self-testing usage is a prerequisite to promote its practical use. The present study reports five experiments exploring whether test anxiety (TA) and test stake (1) affect self-testing usage (Experiments 1-5) and (2) influence learning performance ...

  26. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    Choosing Money Over Meaningful Work: Examining Relative Job Preferences for High Compensation Versus Meaningful Work. Sarah Ward. Preview abstract. Restricted access Research article First published April 20, 2023 pp. 1128-1148. xml GET ACCESS. Table of contents for Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 50, 7, Jul 01, 2024.

  27. What is a Research Hypothesis: How to Write it, Types, and Examples

    Here are some good research hypothesis examples: "The use of a specific type of therapy will lead to a reduction in symptoms of depression in individuals with a history of major depressive disorder.". "Providing educational interventions on healthy eating habits will result in weight loss in overweight individuals.".

  28. Developing a Hypothesis

    Figure 2.3 Hypothetico-Deductive Method Combined With the General Model of Scientific Research in Psychology Together they form a model of theoretically motivated research. As an example, let us consider Zajonc's research on social facilitation and inhibition. ... the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a ...

  29. Review of Soil Creep Characteristics and Advances in Modelling Research

    Creep is recognised to be an important physical property of soils, exerting a profound influence on the stability of structures. In order to gain a comprehensive understanding of the advancements and focal points in soil creep research, the relevant literature was accessed from the Web of Science Core Collection database, totalling 3907 papers (as of 25 March 2024). Statistical analyses on ...