U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Wiley Open Access Collection

Logo of blackwellopen

Dynamics of Identity Development in Adolescence: A Decade in Review

Susan branje.

1 Utrecht University

Elisabeth L. de Moor

Jenna spitzer, andrik i. becht.

2 Erasmus University Rotterdam

One of the key developmental tasks in adolescence is to develop a coherent identity. The current review addresses progress in the field of identity research between the years 2010 and 2020. Synthesizing research on the development of identity, we show that identity development during adolescence and early adulthood is characterized by both systematic maturation and substantial stability. This review discusses the role of life events and transitions for identity and the role of micro‐processes and narrative processes as a potential mechanisms of personal identity development change. It provides an overview of the linkages between identity development and developmental outcomes, specifically paying attention to within‐person processes. It additionally discusses how identity development takes place in the context of close relationships.

In the current review, we discuss progress in the field of identity research between 2010 and 2020. One of the key developmental tasks in adolescence and young adulthood is to develop a coherent sense of self and identity (Erikson, 1968 ). Personal identity refers to one’s sense of the person one genuinely is, including a subjective feeling of self‐sameness and continuity over contexts and time. From early adolescence onwards, adolescents begin to question and explore their identity, that is, the person they are and want to be, the roles they want to occupy in adulthood, and their place in society (Meeus, Van De Schoot, Keijsers, Schwartz, & Branje, 2010 ). They become aware of their distinctiveness and uniqueness from others, coherence and similarity across domains, and continuity across time and situations (van Doeselaar, Becht, Klimstra, & Meeus, 2018 ). As they try to find out who they are and what they want to become in their lives, adolescents re‐examine the identifications they formed in childhood as they consider different identity possibilities (e.g., what kind of occupation fits my interests and abilities, what kind of relationships do I want to have), and form new commitments to identifications.

One way in which adolescents attempt to create continuity and self‐sameness is by integrating their past, present, and future selves into a personal narrative about their lives (McAdams, 2001 ; Pasupathi & Mansour, 2006 ). By constructing a sense of identity in the form of autobiographical stories, adolescents recognize that they are the same person across time and different contexts. Importantly, these life narratives are based on real experiences but are also highly subjective, as adolescents form them according to their own understanding of what was important for who they have become, and are also subject to change, as what adolescents find important for their identity may change over time. Individuals’ life stories differ in certain characteristics that are seen as indicative of identity, such as their level of autobiographical reasoning, complexity, dominant themes, and structure.

Dual‐cycle identity models focus on development of the dynamic identity processes of exploration and commitment that operate along two interrelated cycles. In the identity formation cycle, adolescents consider identity alternatives (i.e., in‐breadth exploration) and form identity commitments. The identity maintenance cycle serves the function to maintain and further strengthen chosen commitments in a dynamic between identity commitment and in‐depth exploration of current commitments. When adolescents’ in‐depth exploration results in increased identity uncertainty about their commitments, they may reconsider their commitments and go back to the identity formation cycle (Crocetti, 2017 ). Moreover, self‐concept clarity, or the extent to which individuals describe themselves consistently and feel sure of themselves (Campbell, 1990 ), has been described as a key ingredient of personal identity (Schwartz et al., 2011 ). Whereas dual‐cycle models of identity account for how the self‐concept is formed, self‐concept clarity might indicate how well the process of developing one’s own identity is going (Schwartz et al., 2011 ), or one’s identity synthesis.

Personal identity concerns self‐sameness and continuity of perceptions of who one is across multiple domains or aspects in life. Salient domains of identity during adolescence are the educational and vocational domain, which involve questions such as which kind of education or profession one wants to obtain, and the relational domain, which involves questions such as what kind of friendships or intimate relationship one wants to have. Other important identities are gender identity, ethnic identity, religious identity, and (mental) illness identity. Although global personal identity reflects the integration of one’s self‐concept across these domains, individuals do not necessarily experience similar levels of self‐sameness and continuity in all domains, particularly in adolescence when identity is still under development. At the same time, experiences in different domains will be intertwined and mutually influence each other. This distinction between global and domain‐specific identity is also apparent in the narratives that individuals construct. Individuals may construe narratives about several life domains, and their narratives may differ depending on the life domain (e.g., Dunlop, 2015 ; Galliher, McLean, & Syed, 2017 ) and on whether these narratives encompass one or multiple life domains (McLean, Syed, Yoder, & Greenhoot, 2016 ).

Personal identity is also strongly intertwined with social identity, which concerns those aspects of individuals’ self‐concept that are derived from the social groups they belong to. Individuals try to maintain a balance between their social identity of belonging to certain groups in comparison with outgroups, and their personal identity, which contains elements of distinctiveness in comparison with other members of the group. Those aspects of social identity that reflect individual’s social orientations or attitudes toward that domain can be considered a part of personal identity (Schwartz, Zamboanga, & Weisskirch, 2008 ). For example, whereas one’s social gender identity reflects the extent to which one perceives oneself as belonging to men, women, or other groups, one’s personal gender identity reflects the extent to which one perceives oneself as unique within the specific gender group. Personal and social identity are likely to reinforce each other and are integrated in the self‐concept (Crocetti, Prati, & Rubini, 2018 ) and identity narrative (Galliher et al., 2017 ). In the current review, we do not focus on social identity per se but include aspects of social identity that are relevant for adolescents’ personal identity, such as their engagement in exploration of their social‐cultural background and forming a clearer sense of what this background or group identity means for their self‐concept.

Even though adolescents increasingly know who they are and who they want to become, identity continues to develop into young adulthood (Schwartz, Côté, & Arnett, 2005 ). Contemporary Western society is characterized by prolonged education and a delay of transitions into adult roles such as work, stable partner relationships, and family life (Schoon, 2015 ). Thus, it might take well into adulthood before individuals reach a mature identity across these various identity domains and are able to create a coherent and relatively stable narrative of their self. The process of establishing a coherent sense of identity can be very stressful. For instance, ongoing identity uncertainty in adolescence and young adulthood has been related to the development of a range of psychosocial adjustment problems (for reviews see Meeus, 2011 ; van Doeselaar, Becht, et al., 2018 ; van Doeselaar, Klimstra, et al., 2018 ). Understanding how and why some individuals develop a coherent identity, whereas others remain highly uncertain about who they are and want to become, is therefore vital for supporting healthy development.

This review aims to synthesize research on the development of personal identity during adolescence. The review will predominantly focus on studies from the perspectives of the dual‐cycle models of identity development (e.g., Crocetti, Rubini, & Meeus, 2008 ; Luyckx, Goossens, & Soenens, 2006 ; Meeus et al., 2010 ) and of narrative identity (McAdams, 2001 ; McLean & Pasupathi, 2012 ), but will also include research using other approaches to personal identity, such as studies focusing on self‐concept clarity or identity processing styles. Narrative processes may be considered one of the key mechanisms of personal identity change (e.g., van Doeselaar, McLean, Meeus, Denissen, & Klimstra, 2020 ; McLean & Pasupathi, 2012 ), and such a synthesis may thus provide more insight into both strands of research, as well as their intersection. This focus aligns with the recently proposed framework for the study of identity development by Galliher et al. ( 2017 ), in which identity is understood as enacted at the micro‐level of real‐time interactions. In these interactions, different personal identity domains are pivotal and intersect, creating an integrated subjective sense of self. These micro‐processes take place within the context of the social roles that adolescents uptake.

By focusing on the underlying processes of identity development, and providing examples from a variety of identity domains, we integrate the current knowledge and offer directions for future research. We cannot give a full account of all the different identity domains, and instead, we focus predominantly on mechanisms underlying personal identity development, thereby giving examples from different identity domains. In our review of the adolescent identity field between 2010 and 2020, we address recent developments on the role of life events and transitions in identity development and discuss the role of within‐person micro‐processes in identity development. We particularly focus on the daily experiences and relational contexts with parents and peers in which identity emerges and is enacted, as these real‐time experiences are the driving mechanisms of intraindividual identity development.

INTRAINDIVIDUAL PROCESSES AND INTERINDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES

Mean level development of identity.

The development of personal identity has predominantly been studied using dual‐cycle models of identity (Crocetti et al., 2008 ; Luyckx et al., 2006 ; Meeus et al., 2010 ). Longitudinal studies have shown that these identity processes mature systematically (for a review on development of commitments see van Doeselaar, Becht, et al., 2018 ; van Doeselaar, Klimstra, et al., 2018 ), but these changes are modest and there is also quite some evidence for stability in identity processes during adolescence and young adulthood. For instance, decreasing mean levels of identity reconsideration in the educational and relational domain indicate that with age, adolescents become more certain about the commitments they have made (Klimstra, Hale, Raaijmakers, Branje, & Meeus, 2010 ), yet mean levels of educational and relational identity commitments are relatively stable in adolescence (Klimstra et al., 2010 ) and young adulthood (Shirai, Nakamura, & Katsuma, 2016 ). In addition, the less adaptive identity statuses of diffusion and moratorium, which are characterized by relatively weak commitments, systematically decrease, while the frequency of the most advanced identity status, identity achievement, increases from early‐to‐late adolescence (Hirschi, 2012 ; Meeus, van de Schoot, Keijsers, & Branje, 2012 ; Meeus et al., 2010 ). Despite these changes, however, the majority of individuals remains in the same identity status during adolescence (ages 12–20, Meeus et al., 2010 ) and young adulthood (ages 25–29, Carlsson, Wängqvist, & Frisén, 2015 ). A study using daily measures of identity in 15 weeks spread across a period of 5 years also found mostly stable patterns: Around 50% of the sample showed relatively high and stable commitment levels, while the other 50% of adolescents experienced a temporal discontinuity of identity commitments, indicated by a dip in commitments during middle adolescence (Becht et al., 2016 ).

Moderate increases during adolescence have been found for self‐concept clarity as well. Although one study showed that self‐concept clarity significantly increased over time in a sample of 12‐ to 21‐year‐olds (Wu, Watkins, & Hattie, 2010 ), this study did not further differentiate age to examine age‐related change patterns. Two other longitudinal studies showed that levels of self‐concept clarity between the ages of 13 and 18 were stable for girls and slightly increasing and then decreasing for boys (Crocetti, Moscatelli, et al., 2016 ) and that self‐concept clarity developed nonlinearly between the ages 17 and 23, with an initial decline from 16 to 17 years of age, followed by an increase until age 23 (Crocetti, Rubini, Branje, Koot, & Meeus, 2016 ).

For development of cultural and gender identity, the evidence thus far also suggests stability combined with some increase. In a sample of Black American boys, the centrality of race and gender to the self and the personal evaluation of one’s race and gender were either stable or decreased (Rogers, Scott, & Way, 2015 ). No developmental changes between the ages 15 to 18 were found in ethnic exploration and belonging of American adolescents with Latinx American, Asian, and European backgrounds (Kiang, Witkow, Baldelomar, & Fuligni, 2010 ). Comparably, among Asian American adolescents followed over a 4‐year period, ethnic Asian American identity remained stable, yet American identity increased over time (Kiang, Witkow, & Champagne, 2013 ).

Overall, these findings show that there is considerable stability in identity synthesis, identity statuses, and the underlying identity processes of commitment, exploration, and reconsideration across adolescence and young adulthood, but when there is developmental change, this change is mostly in the direction of maturation in identity. Also, there is substantial heterogeneity in development of identity processes across individuals and domains, with some adolescents showing changes toward lower self‐certainty and more identity confusion, and some domains showing more stability than others.

Most individuals may not reach a mature identity characterized by an integrated sense of self across multiple identity domains until well into adulthood (Branje & Koper, 2018 ; Kroger, Martinussen, & Marcia, 2010 ). Attesting to this principle of ongoing identity maturation, changes in commitment and exploration processes in educational and interpersonal identity domains have been found to become more strongly intertwined over time into late adolescence (Albarello, Crocetti, & Rubini, 2018 ). Also, young adults (18–23 years of age) showed relatively high identity congruence when comparing developmental patterns of identity commitment across identity domains, such as intimate relationships, parents, and education (Kunnen, 2010 ). That is, 73% of the young adults showed similar patterns in four out of six identity domains, and by young adulthood, most individuals showed an achieved identity as indicated by strong commitments and high levels of exploration across domains. However, only a limited amount of congruence in young adults’ identity statuses was found across vocational and relational identity, with only 18% showing strong commitments in both domains (Luyckx, Seiffge‐Krenke, Schwartz, Crocetti, & Klimstra, 2014 ). Similarly, a second‐order factor analysis showed that global identity processes could only partly explain domain‐specific identity processes in young adulthood (Vosylis, Erentaitė, & Crocetti, 2018 ). Thus, also in young adulthood, it remains important to distinguish between identity domains.

Beyond Mean Level Identity Development

More recently the focus of research has shifted from understanding mean level change to understanding the more complex developmental mechanisms underlying identity maturation. Studies have increasingly focused on intraindividual processes of identity development, and on the transitional periods or salient life events that might require adolescents to reconsider who they are and change their identity. Moreover, in addition to the annual or biannual measures of identity typically employed, micro‐level assessments of identity processes have emerged to offer a more detailed picture of the mechanisms underlying identity development.

Intraindividual associations among identity processes

Research has typically examined developmental changes in identity processes and their associations by focusing on the group level, such as changes in means or interindividual differences. In line with the dual‐cycle model of identity formation, studies have consistently shown negative associations between commitment making and exploration in breadth, positive associations between exploration in depth and identification with commitments, and negative associations between reconsideration and identification with commitments (Waterman, 2015 ). Although these studies offer valuable knowledge about why some individuals develop differently than others, they have not captured the intraindividual processes of identity development, such as whether and how intraindividual changes arise over time. For example, a mean change in identity commitments across adolescents does not tell us how these changes unfold within‐individual adolescents. Additionally, despite relatively stable interindividual differences in adolescent identity, changes within individuals may occur, as processes at the group level are unrelated to processes at the individual level (i.e., at the within‐person level, Molenaar, 2004 ).

The importance of examining intraindividual processes becomes evident when investigating central theories of identity development more closely. These theories in fact describe developmental processes at the intraindividual level; adolescents’ identity formation is expected to take place within individuals across time. For instance, adolescents are thought to experience an identity crisis before developing a clear and stable identity (Erikson, 1968 ). Tests of this hypothesis with interindividual analyses have shown that adolescents who experience more uncertainty about the self than their peers, for example by having weaker commitments and exploring more, typically remain more uncertain than those peers across adolescence (Meeus et al., 2010 ). One might conclude from this research that an identity crisis is not necessary for developing a coherent and mature identity, but these findings do not rule out that individual adolescents go through periods of uncertainty before establishing a more mature identity, irrespective of their level of identity certainty relative to their peers.

In the last decade, research has increasingly focused on how these reorganizations occur within‐individual adolescents. At the within‐person level, although the pattern of concurrent associations is similar to between‐person associations, the over‐time patterns seem to be different. In a 5‐year study among Dutch adolescents (Becht et al., 2017 ), within‐person cross‐lagged models showed that within the same waves, reconsideration was moderately to strongly negatively associated with commitment in the educational and interpersonal domain. That is, in years when adolescents reported higher than usual reconsideration, they also reported lower than usual commitments. Prospectively, when adolescents reported lower educational commitments in a particular year, they reported higher than usual reconsideration in the subsequent year. For interpersonal identity, however, higher reconsideration in a particular year was followed by stronger commitments the next year (Becht et al., 2017 ). Relatedly, within‐person cross‐lagged models indicated that when adolescents scored higher on identity synthesis than usual, they reported higher proactive exploration processes than usual 1 year later (Bogaerts et al., 2019 ). These within‐person findings suggest that reaching a degree of identity synthesis is a prerequisite for proactive identity exploration, and that reconsideration goes hand in hand with weaker commitments, but might be a prerequisite to find new commitments, consistent with the shift from the evaluation cycle to the formation cycle of identity. These findings offer initial support for the Eriksonian idea that commitments result from identity uncertainty.

Development of identity in relation to life transitions and events

Adolescents and young adults develop a sense of self during a phase of life that involves many role changes. These salient transitional periods, such as entering tertiary education, initiating intimate relationships, and joining the workforce, may form an important context that increases the salience of identity processes, and may go hand in hand with identity change. Indeed, much of the narrative tradition is built on the idea that individuals need to integrate important experiences in their lives into their identity (McAdams, 2001 ). Also, identity theory (Bosma & Kunnen, 2008 ; Marcia, 1966 ; Waterman, 1982 ) predicts that identity maturation occurs as the result of individuals’ investment in social roles that require them to engage in mature and socially responsible behavior.

In the last decade, empirical research has increasingly focused on the associations between transitions to new roles and the development of identity. Interindividual differences in identity may affect how individuals navigate through life transitions and take on new social roles. A study of over 1000 adolescents found that educational and relational identity processes in adolescence did not predict involvement in work or an intimate relationship in young adulthood 5 years later, but did predict vocational and relational identity processes in young adulthood (Branje, Laninga‐Wijnen, Yu, & Meeus, 2014 ). These findings suggest that identity processes in adolescence contribute to successful fulfillment of adult roles once these role transitions in young adulthood are made. At the same time, transitions might also affect identity processes. A study on heterogeneity in the development of educational identity across the transition to tertiary education (Christiaens, Nelemans, Meeus, & Branje, 2021 ) revealed that many adolescents show stable patterns of commitment and reconsideration surrounding this transition, yet a sizable minority has either more or less adaptive patterns after the transition, which are meaningfully associated with sociodemographic, academic, individual, and relational characteristics. Thus, transitioning into adult roles may foster the development of the self, but might also confront some adolescents with challenges to their identity.

In addition to normative life transitions, incidental and stressful life events, such as the death of a relative, a breakup of a relationship, or an accident, can serve as turning points in the development of identity (Slotter & Gardner, 2011 ; Waterman, 2020 ). Stressful life events can induce feelings of discontinuity and force adolescents to reconsider their identity. In particular, traumatic events, such as the experience of war, might result in a diminished sense of sameness and continuity of the self (Erikson, 1968 ). This may be reflected in weaker commitments and more exploration and reconsideration especially in identity domains related to the event (Kroger, 1996 ), and an overall sense of identity diffusion (Penner, Gambin, & Sharp, 2019 ). The sense of discontinuity may make individuals reconsider their identity and try to integrate the experienced event into a new sense of identity (Anthis, 2002 ; McAdams, 2001 ). This autobiographical integration of the event into one’s identity is thought to alleviate stress and restore one’s sense of self‐continuity (Habermas & Köber, 2015 ).

Two recent studies examined the longitudinal associations between stressful life events and identity processes. The first study (van Doeselaar, Becht, et al., 2018 ; van Doeselaar, Klimstra, et al., 2018 ) used a two‐sample design and did not find evidence for longitudinal effects between interpersonal commitments and stressful life events in either sample. Although bidirectional negative effects were found between career commitments and stressful life events in one of the samples, only an inconsistent effect of career commitment on subsequent stressful life events was found in the other sample. Thus, adolescents who experienced more negative life events had weaker career commitments, but the evidence for longitudinal effects remains inconsistent. The second study examined within‐person effects of specific life events on identity commitments, exploration, and reconsideration (de Moor, van der Graaff, van Dijk, Meeus, & Branje, 2019 ). Adolescents with fewer commitments and higher levels of reconsideration were more likely to repeat a grade, but the study found no evidence that the experience of a single life event resulted in subsequent changes in identity processes. Hence, while theory suggests that events should be important for identity change, this is not found empirically. A closer focus on how adolescents deal with events and integrate them into their identity may be needed to better understand the role of life events in identity formation.

Micro‐processes of identity

In the last decade, researchers have paid increasing attention to the micro‐level processes that underlie developmental changes in identity commitment and exploration (Lichtwarck‐Aschoff, van Geert, Bosma, & Kunnen, 2008 ). These micro‐level processes refer to the real‐time experiences and expressions of identity in adolescents’ daily lives and are thought to accumulate in the macro‐level development of identity processes (Bosma & Kunnen, 2001 ; Grotevant, 1987 ; Kerpelman, Pittman, & Lamke, 1997 ; Korobov, 2015 ). That is, identity commitments are thought to emerge from the here‐and‐now interactions of adolescents in their daily context. These real‐time micro‐processes are sensitive to context and are more variable than macro‐level identity processes.

To understand the micro‐processes that unfold within individuals over shorter periods of time and their associations with macro‐level identity processes, researchers have developed daily identity measures. A study among 13‐year‐old Dutch adolescents measured daily identity processes for commitment and reconsideration in the interpersonal and educational domain across three 5‐day weeks (Klimstra et al., 2010 ), and found that interindividual differences in daily commitment and reconsideration in the interpersonal identity domain were negatively and reciprocally related, but daily reconsideration in the educational domain negatively predicted next‐day commitment. A study using the same data (Schwartz et al., 2011 ) showed that adolescents with higher levels of reconsideration reported less self‐concept clarity the next day, but self‐concept clarity and commitments were reciprocally associated across days, in which adolescents with stronger global identity commitments on a particular day were more likely to have a clearer self‐concept the next day and vice versa. These studies reveal that day‐to‐day changes in identity processes are associated with each other, and that daily processes differentiate adolescents from each other. How these daily identity processes operate might differ across different identity domains, however.

These findings suggest that especially identity reconsideration might trigger daily change in commitments. When adolescents start reconsidering alternative commitments, it may be functional that the strength of the individual’s current commitment decreases to open‐up the possibility to explore and commit to new identity choices. However, to be able to draw such conclusions regarding how adolescents’ daily identity processes play a role in identity formation at the individual level, we need to investigate intraindividual identity processes at the micro‐level, particularly as such within‐person analyses at the macro‐level suggested that reconsideration was followed by an increase in commitments (Becht et al., 2017 ). Applying an intensive longitudinal design among 31 female university students, van der Gaag, de Ruiter, and Kunnen ( 2016 ) showed that when students reported more than usual daily commitment in the educational domain, they reported less than usual exploration of fit (resembling exploration in depth) and reconsideration, and more than usual exploration of self at the within‐individual level. The negative association between commitment and exploration at the within‐person daily level was consistent with the within‐person association at the annual level (Becht et al., 2017 ). However, the study by Van der Gaag and colleagues also revealed a large amount of heterogeneity between individuals in these within‐person day‐to‐day associations. Although about 50–60% of individuals showed a negative correlation between exploration and commitment, a minority showed a positive or negligible relation. These results stress the importance of examining which individual and contextual factors can explain the large variation that exists in within‐person, micro‐level identity processes.

Even though these studies on micro‐processes of identity have offered new insights into the dynamics of identity development across different time scales, so far micro‐processes have mostly been assessed in a rather abstract and global way, for example by asking whether adolescents felt certain on a particular day regarding their identity. Alternative, more concrete ways to grasp the real‐time experiences that contribute to identity formation are needed, particularly as many identity processes might be unconscious (Schachter, 2018 ). A recent qualitative study examined the concrete micro‐level exploration behaviors adolescents engage in during real‐time interactions with their peers and showed three overarching interaction patterns that advanced exploration: creating a safe environment for exploration, clarifying and elaborating an idea, and a process in which finding a keyword and repeating it helped adolescents explore an aspect of identity and find tentative, emerging commitments (Sugimura, Gmelin, van der Gaag, & Kunnen, 2021 ). Also, among students conducting a clinical internship, real‐time fulfillment versus frustration of the need for competence, relatedness, and autonomy was related to stronger and weaker commitments, respectively (Kunnen, 2021 ). Future research should include such meaningful assessments of the concrete micro‐level behaviors adolescents engage in to form their identity (see also Klimstra & Schwab, 2021 ). Particularly, micro‐level assessments during transitional periods are required to understand identity change.

Daily emotions and identity processes

Daily emotions and experiences may play a particularly important role in the heterogeneity of identity processes, as emotions might have a feedback function to the individual that is relevant to their identity and identity processes in daily life might thus be strongly related to emotions (Kunnen, Bosma, van Halen, & van der Meulen, 2001 ). Using within‐person analyses of daily associations between identity processes and mood in early adolescence, Klimstra et al. ( 2016 ) showed that negative mood was positively associated with educational and interpersonal reconsideration, and weakly positively with in‐depth educational exploration. In addition, negative mood was weakly negatively associated with interpersonal commitment, and strongly negatively with educational commitment. Thus, experiencing negative emotions can elicit uncertainty regarding one’s identity and lead to increased exploration, while experiencing positive emotions can reinforce identity commitments, resulting in increased identification with current identity‐relevant choices. However, the process of identity formation can be stressful and might itself also elicit mood disruptions (Erikson, 1968 ).

In addition to mood, van der Gaag, Albers, and Kunnen ( 2017 ) showed that among first year university students, both positive and negative emotional experiences that were personally important and had impacted their attitude toward their education were related to increases in micro‐level educational commitment. The study used weekly measures of commitment, exploration, and emotional experiences across 22–30 weeks. For most individuals, emotional experiences had a stronger impact on commitment than exploration. Surprisingly, for a minority of students, positive experiences predicted a decrease in educational commitment. As the content of the experiences did not necessarily relate to the domain of education (e.g., forming a romantic relationship), it might be that positive experiences competing with educational commitment resulted in a decrease of educational commitment. Similarly, negative experiences in other domains might result in an increased commitment to one’s education. The finding that negative emotional experiences resulted in increased commitment could also be due to assimilation processes. Following the assimilation‐accommodation perspective (Bosma & Kunnen, 2001 ), a single negative experience might result in assimilation, in which the interpretation of the experience changes to keep the commitment intact. When negative experiences accumulate, individuals may need to accommodate, resulting in a decrease in commitment (Kunnen, 2006 ). Thus, both positive and negative emotional experiences are important in micro‐level educational commitment development. Future research should explore which adolescents are more likely to assimilate their experiences into their identity commitments and how positive and negative emotional experiences within and outside current commitments accumulate to result in identity change.

Narratives as micro‐processes

Narrative processes are also considered micro‐processes that can be studied to examine how individuals engage in commitment and exploration processes. For instance, individuals’ narratives about personally relevant moments in their lives (e.g., low points, high points, and turning points) are thought to be important accounts of continuity or change in their identity (Kunnen & Bosma, 2000 ). Moreover, the function of narratives of creating continuity (McAdams, 2001 ; Pasupathi & Mansour, 2006 ) is thought to be related to establishing commitments (Erikson, 1968 ). For example, young adults with gender narratives reflecting equality positions reported higher identity exploration and lower commitment than young adults with traditional gender narratives, suggesting that identity exploration is related to engagement with alternative narratives and that engagement with traditional narratives is related to conventionality comparable to identity foreclosure (McLean, Shucard, & Syed, 2017 ). Also, increases in young adults’ ethnic identity commitments were not only related to changes in narrative themes, but also to changes in narratives about prejudice or connection to culture (Syed & Azmitia, 2010 ).

Forming commitments can also change the narratives that individuals construe. For example, Swedish young adults who were in identity achievement generally reported a deepening of their life narrative across 4 years, in terms of the way they derived meaning from new experiences, handled changes, and determined their own direction in life (Carlsson et al., 2015 ). However, no clear pattern of narrative development was found for young adults in the stably committed status of identity foreclosure. Also, in a sample of Dutch adolescents, autobiographical reasoning and agency were found to be related to commitment and exploration processes both cross‐sectionally and longitudinally (van Doeselaar et al., 2020 ). However, the size of these associations was small, indicating that narratives and dual‐cycle processes may capture different parts of adolescents’ identity. Thus, narrative processes might be linked to processes of commitment and exploration and might form the underlying mechanism behind identity development more broadly. Future research should further explore how these different approaches toward studying identity development can be used to complement and extend one another.

The role of variability in the development of the self

Another aspect of micro‐level processes relevant for identity development is the amount of variability in identity processes, or the within‐person fluctuations in commitments and exploration across days and weeks. A dynamic system approach suggests that identity micro‐processes fluctuate and change on a daily basis (Lichtwarck‐Aschoff et al., 2008 ). Developmental changes are thought to be nonlinear (Lewis, 2000 ) and may particularly occur at times of transitions or life events that motivate identity development. These transitional periods are thought to be marked by a substantial temporary increase in intraindividual variability (Lichtwarck‐Aschoff et al., 2008 ), or fluctuations in identity processes, after which a restabilization occurs and a more mature identity emerges. Indeed, when adolescents showed a temporal increase in their educational commitment fluctuations across days (i.e., more ups and downs in their commitments), they reported higher educational commitment levels 1 year later (Becht et al., 2017 ). These temporal fluctuations might indicate a period of active reflection on present commitments to support identity development. Identity processes also have been found to be meaningfully related to variability in identity salience among ethnically diverse adolescents (Wang, Douglass, & Yip, 2017 ). Adolescents with stronger ethnic–racial identity commitment reported higher daily mean salience and less variability in salience 6 months later, and adolescents who reported more daily variability in salience engaged in more exploration 6 months later, suggesting that daily experiences might trigger exploration and that commitment might be consolidated in daily ethnic salience experiences. Thus, variability in micro‐processes of identity may be the underlying mechanism that marks the dynamic of identity development.

Although higher variability may be necessary for individuals to adapt and reorganize themselves effectively in response to varying contextual changes (Moskowitz & Zuroff, 2004 ), higher levels of variability might also indicate behavioral lability, or variability in behavior that is not well‐controlled or very adaptive (Charles & Pasupathi, 2003 ). So far, with the exception of Becht et al. ( 2017 ) and Wang et al. ( 2017 ), higher levels of fluctuations in identity processes have mostly been associated with poorer outcomes in several domains. Stronger fluctuations in reconsideration (but not commitment or self‐concept clarity) across days were associated with lower levels of macro‐level interpersonal and educational commitments and higher levels of reconsideration (Klimstra et al., 2010 ), as well as with later symptoms of anxiety and depression, controlling for previous levels of anxiety and depression (Schwartz et al., 2011 ). These findings suggest that reconsideration might particularly create a sense of disequilibrium and distress when this sense of uncertainty fluctuates across days, regardless of the level of reconsideration.

Thus, while these findings mostly suggest that variability in identity processes is negatively related to psychosocial functioning, stronger increases in variability in specific transitional periods may also reflect developmental changes in identity. Variability in transitional periods might be associated with poorer well‐being because individuals experience the period as stressful, resulting temporarily in declines in adjustment. To shed light on the role of fluctuations in identity processes in macro‐development of identity, future research should examine variability in identity and self‐concept clarity during transitional and nontransitional periods and link this variability to developmental processes in identity.

In sum, instead of focusing on interindividual differences, recent studies have sought to analyze identity processes at the intraindividual, micro‐level—that is, at the level where development is taking place: within the person, in here‐and‐now experiences. Knowledge of these micro‐level processes might be particularly informative for interventions in youth who struggle with identity development. For example, Blaauw et al. ( 2019 ) recently developed the u‐can‐act platform, a tool to study individual processes of identity development in the context of early school leaving and the preventative behaviors of youth’s mentors to help youth cope with their educational identity development at individual level.

IDENTITY AND PSYCHOSOCIAL ADJUSTMENT

Identity is quite consistently related to a broad range of psychosocial adjustment outcomes across cultures, underlining the importance of identity in adolescents’ lives. Adolescents with stable and strong identity commitments have consistently been found to report higher levels of psychosocial adjustment over time than adolescents with ongoing identity uncertainty (Crocetti et al., 2008 ; Hatano, Sugimura, & Schwartz, 2018 ; Meeus et al., 2010 ). Prior studies revealed that adolescents with a weaker sense of identity and ongoing reconsideration of identity alternatives are at increased risk for the development of aggressive behavior (Becht et al., 2016 ; Crocetti, Klimstra, Hale, Koot, & Meeus, 2013 ; Morsunbul, 2015 ), delinquent behavior (Levey, Garandeau, Meeus, & Branje, 2019 ), and substance use (de Moor, Sijtsema, Weller, & Klimstra, 2020 ). Similarly, adolescents with ongoing identity uncertainty were more likely to show increasing (school) anxiety levels over time (Becht et al., 2016 ), higher loneliness (Kaniušonytė, Truskauskaitė‐Kunevičienė, Žukauskienė, & Crocetti, 2019 ), and higher levels of depressive symptoms (van Doeselaar, Becht, et al., 2018 ; van Doeselaar, Klimstra, et al., 2018 ; Meeus et al., 2012 ).

In addition to negative psychosocial adjustment, individual differences in dual‐cycle identity processes predict positive adjustment as well. For instance, those individuals with strong and stable identity commitments report higher self‐esteem (Luyckx et al., 2013 ) and higher levels of experienced meaning in life (Negru‐Subtirica, Pop, Luyckx, Dezutter, & Steger, 2016 ). Also, adolescents with an ethnic identity profile characterized by high exploration, strong resolution (resembling commitments), and high centrality of their ethnic–racial background within their self‐concept reported higher academic engagement, life satisfaction, and self‐esteem than adolescents with an ethnic identity profile characterized by low exploration and resolution (regardless of centrality; Wantchekon & Umaña‐Taylor, 2021 ). Although ethnic–racial identity exploration did not predict civic beliefs, adolescents who reported greater increases in ethnic–racial identity resolution across 2 years of middle school did report greater increases in civic beliefs on the need to advance the well‐being of their communities (Bañales, Hoffman, Rivas‐Drake, & Jagers, 2020 ). Moreover, a meta‐analysis showed that stronger commitments and composite scores of ethnic–racial identity processes can buffer the associations of discrimination with adjustment problems, yet higher levels of exploration increased associations of discrimination with adjustment problems (Yip, Wang, Mootoo, & Mirpuri, 2019 ). Furthermore, identity processes have been linked to academic and career functioning. The development of career adaptability and vocational identity commitment are dynamically intertwined across adolescence (Negru‐Subtirica, Pop, & Crocetti, 2015 ). Identity development in college students positively affected their sense of competence and motivational beliefs to master their study (Perez, Cromley, & Kaplan, 2014 ). In contrast, higher academic achievement predicted higher levels of commitment and lower levels of reconsideration (Pop, Negru‐Subtirica, Crocetti, Opre, & Meeus, 2016 ), and school engagement positively predicted information‐oriented identity processing, while school burnout positively predicted the use of normative and diffuse‐avoidant identity styles (Erentaitė, Vosylis, Gabrialavičiūtė, & Raižienė, 2018 ).

Although evidence for the directionality of effects between identity processes and psychosocial and academic adjustment is not consistent, these findings suggest that intervening in identity processes might be an auspicious way to promote adolescents’ well‐being. Indeed, the Identity Project intervention, which was designed for delivery in high school for youth in middle adolescence to target ethnic–racial identity exploration and resolution, showed promising effects on adolescents’ identity processes and adjustment (Umaña‐Taylor, Douglass, Updegraff, & Marsiglia, 2018 ; Umaña‐Taylor et al., 2018 ). The intervention consisted of eight sessions that were designed to help adolescents understand between‐ and within‐group differences and how various groups had been marginalized throughout history, and to help adolescents engage in activities to facilitate the exploration of their ethnic–racial background. The Identity Project intervention resulted in increases in adolescents’ ethnic–racial identity exploration and, subsequently, increases in resolution or sense of clarity of their ethnic–racial identity (Umaña‐Taylor, Douglass, et al., 2018 ). In turn, these changes in exploration and resolution were related to adolescents’ higher global identity cohesion, lower depressive symptoms, higher self‐esteem, and better grades 1 year later (Umaña‐Taylor et al., 2018).

Characteristics of adolescents’ narrative identity are also linked to psychosocial adjustment. The various narrative characteristics that have been studied in relation to adjustment can be categorized in one of three broader factors (Adler, Lodi‐Smith, Philippe, & Houle, 2016 ; McLean et al., 2020 ): autobiographical reasoning (or integrative meaning), motivational and affective themes, and structure. Autobiographical reasoning refers to individuals’ ability to reflect on their past, present, and future, and link these three together into a continuous narrative (Habermas & Bluck, 2000 ; McAdams, 2001 ). The making of self‐event connections reflects individuals’ autobiographical attempts to explicitly link an event to an aspect of their self (Pasupathi, Mansour, & Brubaker, 2007 ). Adolescents with better‐developed autobiographical reasoning and more self‐event connections report higher well‐being (van Doeselaar et al., 2020 ; McLean, Breen, & Fournier, 2010 ). Initial findings further suggest that this association might vary by developmental period: Although autobiographical reasoning is related to better well‐being in late adolescence, early adolescents who engage in autobiographical reasoning reported lower well‐being than peers who do not (McLean et al., 2010 ; McLean & Mansfield, 2011 ). Moreover, different themes may be dominant in identity narratives, such as redemption (McAdams, Reynolds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001 ), agency, and communion (Adler, 2012 ; McAdams, 2001 ). Adolescents who have identity narratives high on themes of redemption, agency, and communion tend to report higher well‐being (van Doeselaar et al., 2020 ; Dunlop, Harake, Gray, Hanley, & McCoy, 2018 ; Holm, Thomsen, & Bliksted, 2018 ; McAdams & McLean, 2013 ). Finally, when comparing the structure of individuals’ narratives in clinical and population samples, narrative coherence is generally lower in clinical samples (Adler, Chin, Kolisetty, & Oltmanns, 2012 ), and individuals who write more coherent narratives report fewer psychological difficulties and greater well‐being (Reese et al., 2011 ; Waters & Fivush, 2015 ; Waters, Shallcross, & Fivush, 2013 ). This might show that identity in the initial stages of development might go together with uncertainty and lower adjustment.

In sum, there is a vast body of literature highlighting the associations between personal identity development and psychosocial outcomes, providing evidence that adolescents with a more well‐adjusted identity tend to fair better in many different areas of life. Conversely, this research underlines how adolescents with issues in their identity may be stuck in broader maladaptive development. Findings concerning the direction of effects are less consistent. Probably, different processes play a role for different groups of youth: In some, identity synthesis might result in better adjustment, in others adjustment problems might trigger identity uncertainty, and yet in others, external factors might result in a change in both identity and adjustment. More work is needed examining the direction of effects, particularly at the within‐person level, to gain a better understanding of developmental processes linking identity and adjustment and to improve interventions aimed at strengthening adolescents’ identity processes.

Within‐Person Associations of Identity and Psychosocial Adjustment

In the last decade, research has started to examine these within‐person linkages between identity development and psychosocial adjustment (Lichtwarck‐Aschoff et al., 2008 ). To this end, Becht et al. ( 2019 ) examined the intraindividual longitudinal linkages between identity and depressive symptoms in two large longitudinal Dutch and Belgian adolescent samples. In both samples, a within‐person increase in identity uncertainty predicted a within‐person increase in depressive symptoms 1 year later but not vice versa. These findings suggest a role of ongoing identity uncertainty in the development of depressive symptoms over time. Similarly, Mercer, Crocetti, Branje, van Lier, and Meeus ( 2017 ) showed bidirectional intraindividual linkages between identity and delinquency, such that increasing delinquency predicted weaker identity commitments and more identity reconsideration and vice versa. When adolescents showed an increase of in‐depth exploration, their delinquency levels decreased 1 year later. Together, these findings suggest that identity struggles might result in feelings of despair and ways of coping that help adolescents find an alternative (delinquent or negative) identity.

More generally, these findings speak to the importance of considering the content of adolescents’ identity. In some cases, youth may develop a negative identity in which they identify with roles opposed to societal expectations (Hihara, Sugimura, & Syed, 2018 ), and psychopathology or deviancy may even become the content of identity (Cruwys & Gunaseelan, 2016 ). For instance, rejection of delinquent behaviors within society may further strengthen delinquent or negative identities for some youth, as illustrated in the example of radicalization of youth (Meeus, 2015 ). Critically, the longitudinal associations between identity processes and psychosocial adjustment might depend on the content of identity. For example, adolescents with achieved but content‐wise distinct ethnic–racial identities might show different levels of adjustment on individual and group‐relevant outcomes (Umaña‐Taylor et al., 2014 ). Comparably, a recently proposed framework for understanding the associations of identity and psychopathology points out the many alternative ways in which identity and adjustment may be connected and suggests that when identity formation goes awry, psychopathology may become part of one’s identity (Klimstra & Denissen, 2017 ).

More broadly, individuals’ identities should be understood within the context in which adolescents are embedded. Syed, Juang, and Svensson ( 2018 ) provide a comprehensive theoretical framework for understanding ethnic–racial identity within ethnic–racial settings, or the objective and subjective nature of group representation within an individual’s context. Similarly, identity narratives are shaped by the culture in which adolescents live (e.g., McAdams, 2001 , 2006 ). The master narrative of the culture may provide important clues to individual members about how their narrative identity is supposed to look like in terms of structure and themes (for an integrative framework, see McLean & Syed, 2016 ). This culture may also moderate the associations between young people’s narratives and their well‐being (Eriksson, McLean, & Ann Frisén, 2020 ; McLean & Syed, 2016 ). However, so far this work was situated in adult samples, and more research is needed to extend these findings toward adolescents.

IDENTITY AND RELATIONSHIPS

The development of a coherent sense of self and identity takes place in the context of close relationships, such as with family members and peers. Identity concerns not only the experience of self‐sameness and continuity of the self, but also the extent to which this experience corresponds with the sameness and continuity of one’s meaning to significant others. Interactions with family members and friends are therefore crucial. In these interactions, adolescents receive self‐relevant feedback (Grotevant & Cooper, 1985 ; Koepke & Denissen, 2012 ), which they can use to strengthen or adjust their identity commitments. Supportive relationships with family members and friends who are sensitive and responsive to adolescents’ changing need also form a secure base from which adolescents can confidently explore identity options. In interactions with close others, adolescents narrate about their life events and experiences and link these experiences to who they are and how they want to become. These interactions may help adolescents make sense of their experiences and ultimately create more coherent and meaningful identity narratives (e.g., through scaffolding behavior; Fivush, Reese, & Haden, 2006 ). For example, when young adults and their conversation partners agreed about the meaning that could be derived from the narrative they told, they were more likely to retain that meaning for a longer period (McLean & Pasupathi, 2011 ). In addition, as adolescents develop a clearer sense of identity, they are better able to engage in positive interactions with others (Swann, 2000 ; Swann, Milton, & Polzer, 2000 ).

Autonomy support is considered particularly important for identity development. As adolescents start to develop their identity, they also begin to renegotiate autonomy in their relationships with parents and peers. Parents and adolescents have to reallocate responsibilities and power in their relationships to become more egalitarian, with more reciprocity and equality in exchanges, power, and decision‐making (Branje, Laursen, & Collins, 2012 ; de Goede, Branje, & Meeus, 2009 ). Adolescents’ relationships with friends also change toward greater autonomy. Toward middle adolescence, their increased perspective‐taking skills and greater self‐certainty allow friends to respect each other’s individuality without jeopardizing their mutual sense of connectedness (Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011 ; Selfhout, Branje, & Meeus, 2009 ). Friendships also become more supportive over the course of adolescence (de Goede, Branje, Delsing, et al., 2009 ). When parents are unable to release control and adapt toward a more reciprocal relationship, and when friends have difficulties accepting each other’s individuation, adolescents might be likely to develop a less optimal identity, characterized by internal conflict and feelings of alienation from one’s sense of self.

Family Relationships and Identity Processes

Indeed, several recent studies provided evidence for the role of relationships with family members in identity development. Sugimura et al. ( 2018 ) found consistent negative associations of emotional separation and positive associations of parental trust with identity consolidation across Lithuania, Italy, and Japan. Although the identity processes commitment and reconsideration were reciprocally associated with support and conflict in relationships with parents and siblings in a six‐wave study spanning from age 13 to 18 (Crocetti et al., 2017 ), commitment and in‐depth exploration were more likely to positively affect relationship quality with parents and siblings than vice versa. Reconsideration was predicted by low levels of maternal support and worsened the quality of the paternal relationship. Parental support has also been found to be related to higher levels of adolescents’ exploration of their romantic identity (Pittman, Kerpelman, Soto, & Adler‐Baeder, 2012 ). Parental psychological control had a more complex and indirect relation with romantic identity: It was related to an elevated feeling of both avoidance and anxiety in romantic relationships, and while avoidance was in turn related to less exploration of romantic identity, anxiety was related to more exploration. On the level of parent–child interactions, adolescents whose parents stimulate them in their story‐telling to derive meaning from experiences have been found to engage in more autobiographical reasoning (McLean & Jennings, 2012 ). For ethnic identity, parental ethnic socialization was positively related to adolescents’ ethnic identity exploration and resolution (Hu, Zhou, & Lee, 2017 ), and significantly predicted ethnic identity exploration and commitment 1 year later (Else‐Quest & Morse, 2015 ). Moreover, at the within‐person level, increases in family cohesion were associated with increases in ethnic belonging (Kiang et al., 2010 ). These findings confirm that adolescents’ identity development is strengthened in the context of close and supportive family relationships. In addition vice versa, adolescents who have higher commitment and in‐depth exploration also improve their relationships with their family members. Adolescents’ optimal development of identity goes together with high levels of closeness and relatedness.

In addition to parent–adolescent relationship quality and parenting characteristics, parents’ own identities might affect adolescents’ identity development (Schachter, 2018 ). Among Roma minority families, ethnic identity of both parents was strongly and positively associated with adolescent ethnic identity (Dimitrova, Ferrer‐Wreder, & Trost, 2015 ). In a longitudinal study from early‐to‐late adolescence, parental self‐concept clarity was found to unidirectionally predict adolescents’ self‐concept clarity (Crocetti, Moscatelli, et al., 2016 ). Also, caregiver identity commitment was significantly related to adolescent identity distress, over and above adolescents’ own identity commitment and exploration (Wiley & Berman, 2012 ). Therefore, parents with a more coherent identity not only provide an example for adolescents as to how they can explore self‐relevant issues, but they also are better able to provide their children with support as they explore those issues and make commitments.

Peer Relationships and Identity Processes

Although significantly less research has been conducted in this area, peer relationships are also positively related to identity development (for a review, see Ragelienė, 2016 ). In fact, a recent study found that especially support from friends, compared to support from parents, was related to relational and educational identity (de Moor et al., 2019 ). Having a high‐quality relationship with one’s best friend has also been associated with making redemption sequences and self‐event connections in one’s identity narrative (de Moor, van der Graaff, van Doeselaar, Klimstra, & Branje, 2021 ), both of which are seen as characteristics of well‐adjusted narratives. On the level of interactions, adolescents whose friends listen more actively and who stimulate meaning‐making tend to engage in greater autobiographical reasoning (McLean & Jennings, 2012 ; Pasupathi & Hoyt, 2009 ). A 4‐year study of adolescents aged 14–18 years revealed that autonomy support from friends significantly predicted adolescents' reconsideration, and was predicted by in‐depth exploration and, although less consistent, commitment (van Doeselaar, Meeus, Koot, & Branje, 2016 ). These findings suggest that higher autonomy support from a friend reduces adolescents' problematic educational reconsideration, and adaptive educational identity processes foster autonomy‐supportive interactions. A study applying within‐person cross‐lagged analyses indicated that although relationship quality with parents—and not with friends—affected subsequent self‐concept clarity, self‐concept clarity affected subsequent relationship quality with both parents and friends, although results varied across reporters (Becht et al., 2017 ). Thus, higher friendship quality facilitates identity formation, and adaptive identity processes foster supportive and high‐quality interactions.

The development of personal identity is also strongly embedded within the context of the broader peer group. Adolescents’ identification with their classmates and with their group of friends over time predicted stronger interpersonal identity commitment and exploration, as well as lower reconsideration (Albarello et al., 2018 ). These findings suggest that when adolescents are comfortable and at ease in their larger peer groups, they have a basis from which they can explore their personal identity.

Identity is not only directly related to relationship quality; it also affects how susceptible youth are to the influence of others. Adolescents with lower self‐concept clarity were more susceptible to the influence of their friends’ delinquency and subsequently showed more delinquency themselves (Levey et al., 2019 ). Similarly, in more controlling peer groups, adolescents with stronger identity commitment engaged in less risk behavior than adolescents with weaker identity commitment (Dumas, Ellis, & Wolfe, 2012 ), suggesting that identity might protect against peer pressure. A clearer identity might help adolescents compare their views of themselves and the world to those of their peers, while at the same time differentiating themselves as a separate, autonomous person. As a result, they may have greater respect for their own boundaries and be able to resist negative peer influences.

In sum, adolescents who do not experience closeness or supportiveness in their relationships with parents and peers, or adolescents whose parents and peers do not accept their individual needs, tend to experience more difficulties in developing a clear identity. In the process of discovering their own identity and negotiating their needs for autonomy, these adolescents might become overly dependent on others or, alternatively, struggle with feelings of dependence. This may especially be the case in Western cultures, because individuality is emphasized and dependence on others tends to be viewed as a sign of weakness that should be discouraged. Although most research has focused on the role of relationships with parents and friends in adolescents’ identity formation, other relationships offer an important relational context for identity development as well. For example, adolescents with avoidant or anxious attachment, who are less comfortable with romantic partners, were found to have lower relational identity commitments and a higher tendency to use a diffuse/avoidant style for identity exploration (Kerpelman et al., 2012 ), and the extent to which teachers serve as role models predicts student identity development (Rich & Schachter, 2012 ).

CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

The current review discussed progress in the field of adolescent personal identity research between 2010 and 2020. Concerning mean level change, research in this decade has further confirmed that there is considerable stability in identity across adolescence and young adulthood. At the same time, there is substantial heterogeneity in identity development, and when there is developmental change, this change is in the direction of identity maturation.

Excitingly, studies have increasingly begun to go beyond the study of mean level identity change and examine the underlying mechanisms of identity development. It should be noted that many of the developments discussed in this review are emerging developments in the field of identity research, and more research is needed to replicate these findings and provide more knowledge on the robustness and strength of the findings. Nevertheless, these findings suggest identity development requires a focus on transitional periods that pose a challenge to adolescents’ personal identity, as well as a focus on the micro‐processes of identity development. Whereas theory suggests that life transitions and life events might trigger identity change, so far not much empirical support for the effect of life events on personal identity has been found. Future research should examine identity development across life transitions and compare personal identity processes before, during, and after the transition. As the most used identity measures tap into quite global aspects of identity that might not always optimally reflect the process of personal identity formation (Waterman, 2015 ), measures tapping into more concrete commitment making and exploration processes are needed. Also, a closer focus on how adolescents deal with events and integrate them into their identity may be needed to better understand the role of life events in identity. So far, results suggest that micro‐level and macro‐level identity processes tap into qualitatively different processes. To further enhance our knowledge on how micro‐level processes shape macro‐level identity development, future research needs to incorporate the assessment of micro‐level processes in long‐term longitudinal studies that assess processes at the macro‐level.

Recent findings on the associations of identity with psychosocial adjustment confirm that certainty about oneself and the direction one is going in is closely related to better functioning in multiple domains. Increased attention to the intraindividual associations between identity and adjustment (Lichtwarck‐Aschoff et al., 2008 ) shows how within‐person developmental processes in personal identity might affect future adjustment and well‐being and suggests that ongoing identity uncertainty might result in feelings of despair and less adaptive ways of coping. In future research, we need to consider the content of adolescents’ identity in addition to the process of identity formation, as psychopathology may become the content of identity when identity formation goes awry (Klimstra & Denissen, 2017 ).

Research on the social context of identity shows that identity development is closely embedded in relationships with family members and friends. Particularly, parents have an important role in shaping adolescent identity: Instead of turning away from parents, adolescents’ development of identity is fostered by high levels of closeness and relatedness to parents. In addition, parents’ own identities also affect adolescents’ identity development. Although peers seem to have a weaker influence on adolescent identity development compared to parents, a more mature identity helps adolescents establish high‐quality friendships. Identity is not only directly related to relationship quality; it also affects how susceptible youth are to the influence of peers, as adolescents with a more uncertain identity tend to be more vulnerable to negative peer influences.

Last, research is starting to empirically reunify the narrative and dual‐cycle identity approach (e.g., van Doeselaar et al., 2020 ; McLean & Pasupathi, 2012 ), showing how narrative processes may be considered one of the key mechanisms of identity change, providing information on real‐time identity processes. Excitingly, research on narrative processes has also recently started to examine factors such as developmental contexts and content of narratives that moderate the narrative identity–well‐being link. To further this work, it is important that more studies adopt an approach that examines multiple narratives by the same individual, to understand better what aspects of narrative identity are specific to certain narratives, and which are true across narratives and life domains. Although several studies already do so (e.g., Lilgendahl & McLean, 2020 ; McLean, Syed, Yoder, et al., 2016 ), broader implementation of this approach is likely to yield exciting new, robust, and generalizable discoveries. Also, future research could examine narratives of adolescents prompted to discuss the same experience, such as a school transition, to allow for greater comparability between individuals and to enable a greater focus on the actual content of the narrative. Linking such narratives to long‐term intraindividual identity formation processes will increase our understanding of the underlying real‐time mechanisms in personal identity formation and offer more concrete tools for intervention.

This work was supported by a grant from the European Research Council (ERC‐CoG INTRANSITION‐773023).

  • Adler, J. M. (2012). Living into the story: Agency and coherence in a longitudinal study of narrative identity development and mental health over the course of psychotherapy . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 102 , 367–389. 10.1037/a0025289 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Adler, J. M. , Chin, E. D. , Kolisetty, A. P. , & Oltmanns, T. F. (2012). The distinguishing characteristics of narrative identity in adults with features of borderline personality disorder: An empirical investigation . Journal of Personality Disorders , 26 , 498–512. 10.1521/pedi.2012.26.4.498 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Adler, J. M. , Lodi‐Smith, J. , Philippe, F. L. , & Houle, I. (2016). The incremental validity of narrative identity in predicting well‐being: A review of the field and recommendations for the future . Personality and Social Psychology Review , 20 , 142–175. 10.1177/1088868315585068 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Albarello, F. , Crocetti, E. , & Rubini, M. (2018). I and us: A longitudinal study on the interplay of personal and social identity in adolescence . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 47 , 689–702. 10.1007/s10964-017-0791-4 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anthis, K. S. (2002). On the calamity theory of growth: The relationship between stressful life events and changes in identity over time . Identity , 2 , 229–240. 10.1207/S1532706XID0203_03 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bañales, J. , Hoffman, A. J. , Rivas‐Drake, D. , & Jagers, R. J. (2020). The development of ethnic‐racial identity process and its relation to civic beliefs among Latinx and Black American adolescents . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 49 , 2495–2508. 10.1007/s10964-020-01254-6 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becht, A. I. , Luyckx, K. , Nelemans, S. A. , Goossens, L. , Branje, S. J. T. , Vollebergh, W. A. M. , & Meeus, W. H. J. (2019). Linking identity and depressive symptoms across adolescence: A multisample longitudinal study testing within‐person effects . Developmental Psychology , 55 , 1733–1742. 10.1037/dev0000742 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becht, A. I. , Nelemans, S. A. , Branje, S. J. , Vollebergh, W. A. , Koot, H. M. , Denissen, J. J. , & Meeus, W. H. (2016). The quest for identity in adolescence: Heterogeneity in daily identity formation and psychosocial adjustment across 5 years . Developmental Psychology , 52 , 2010–2021. 10.1037/dev0000245 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becht, A. , Nelemans, S. , Van Dijk, M. P. A. , Branje, S. J. T. , van Lier, P. A. C. , Denissen, J. , & Meeus, W. H. J. (2017). Clear self, better relationships: Adolescents' self‐concept clarity and relationship quality with parents and peers across five-years . Child Development , 88 , 1823–1833. 10.1111/cdev.12921 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blaauw, F. J. , van der Gaag, M. , Snell, N. R. , Emerencia, A. C. , Kunnen, E. S. , & de Jonge, P. (2019). The u‐can‐act platform: A tool to study intra‐individual processes of early school leaving and its prevention using multiple informants . Frontiers in Psychology , 10 , 1808. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01808 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bogaerts, A. , Claes, L. , Schwartz, S. J. , Becht, A. I. , Verschueren, M. , Gandhi, A. , & Luyckx, K. (2019). Identity structure and processes in adolescence: Examining the directionality of between‐and within‐person associations . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 48 ( 5 ), 891–907. 10.1007/s10964-018-0931-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bosma, H. A. , & Kunnen, E. S. (2001). Determinants and mechanisms in ego identity development: A review and synthesis . Developmental Review , 21 , 39–66. 10.1006/drev.2000.0514 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bosma, H. A. , & Kunnen, E. S. (2008). Identity‐in‐context is not yet identity development‐in‐context . Journal of Adolescence , 31 , 281–289. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.03.001 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Branje, S. , & Koper, N. (2018). Psychosocial development. In Bornstein M. (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development . Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc., 1772–1774. 10.4135/9781506307633 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Branje, S. , Laninga‐Wijnen, L. , Yu, R. , & Meeus, W. (2014). Associations among school and friendship identity in adolescence and romantic relationships and work in emerging adulthood . Emerging Adulthood , 2 , 6–16. 10.1177/2167696813515851 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Branje, S. , Laursen, B. , & Collins, W. A. (2012). Parent‐child communication during adolescence . Routledge Handbook of Family Communication , 2 , 271–286. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brechwald, W. A. , & Prinstein, M. J. (2011). Beyond homophily: A decade of advances in understanding peer influence processes . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 21 , 166–179. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00721 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self‐esteem and clarity of the self‐concept . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 59 , 538–549. https://doi‐org/10.1037/0022‐3514.59.3.538 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carlsson, J. , Wängqvist, M. , & Frisén, A. (2015). Identity development in the late twenties: A never ending story . Developmental Psychology , 51 , 334–345. 10.1037/a0038745 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charles, S. T. , & Pasupathi, M. (2003). Age‐related patterns of variability in self‐descriptions: Implications for everyday affective experience . Psychology and Aging , 18 , 524–536. 10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.524 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Christiaens, A. H. , Nelemans, S. A. , Meeus, W. H. , & Branje, S. (2021). Identity development across the transition from secondary to tertiary education: A 9‐wave longitudinal study . Journal of Adolescence . 10.1016/j.adolescence.2021.03.007 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. (2017). Identity formation in adolescence: The dynamic of forming and consolidating identity commitments . Child Development Perspectives , 11 , 145–150. 10.1111/cdep.12226 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. , Klimstra, T. A. , Hale, W. W. , Koot, H. M. , & Meeus, W. (2013). Impact of early adolescent externalizing problem behaviors on identity development in middle to late adolescence: A prospective 7‐year longitudinal study . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 42 , 1745–1758. 10.1007/s10964-013-9924-6 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. , Moscatelli, S. , Van der Graaff, J. , Rubini, M. , Meeus, W. , & Branje, S. (2016). The interplay of self‐certainty and prosocial development in the transition from late adolescence to emerging adulthood . European Journal of Personality , 30 , 594–607. 10.1002/per.2084 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. , Branje, S. , Rubini, M. , Koot, H. & Meeus, W. (2017). Identity processes and parent‐child and sibling relationships in adolescence: A five‐wave multi‐informant longitudinal study . Child Development , 88 , 210–228. 10.1111/cdev.12547 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. , Prati, F. , & Rubini, M. (2018). The interplay of personal and social identity . European Psychologist , 23 , 300–310. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000336 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. , Rubini, M. , Branje, S. , Koot, H. M. , & Meeus, W. (2016). Self‐concept clarity in adolescents and parents: A six‐wave longitudinal and multi‐informant study on development and intergenerational transmission . Journal of Personality , 84 , 580–593. 10.1111/jopy.12181 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti, E. , Rubini, M. , & Meeus, W. (2008). Capturing the dynamics of identity formation in various ethnic groups: Development and validation of a three‐dimensional model . Journal of Adolescence , 31 , 207–222. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.09.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cruwys, T. , & Gunaseelan, S. (2016). “Depression is who I am”: Mental illness identity, stigma and wellbeing . Journal of Affective Disorders , 189 , 36–42. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.012 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Goede, I. H. , Branje, S. J. , Delsing, M. J. , & Meeus, W. H. (2009). Linkages over time between adolescents' relationships with parents and friends . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 38 , 1304–1315. 10.1007/s10964-009-9403-2 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Goede, I. H. , Branje, S. J. , & Meeus, W. H. (2009). Developmental changes in adolescents’ perceptions of relationships with their parents . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 38 , 75–88. 10.1007/s10964-008-9286-7 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Moor, E. L. , Sijtsema, J. J. , Weller, J. A. , & Klimstra, T. A. (2020). Longitudinal links between identity and substance use in adolescence . Self and Identity . 10.1080/15298868.2020.1818615 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Moor, E. L. , van der Graaff, J. , van Dijk, M. P. A. , Meeus, W. , & Branje, S. (2019). Stressful life events and identity development in early and mid‐adolescence . Journal of Adolescence , 76 , 75–87. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.08.006 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Moor, E. L. , van der Graaff, J. , van Doeselaar, L. , Klimstra, T. A. , & Branje, S. (2021). With a little help from my friends? Perceived friendship quality and narrative identity in adolescence . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 31 , 384–401. 10.1111/jora.12605 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dimitrova, R. , Ferrer‐Wreder, L. , & Trost, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of ethnic identity and life satisfaction of Roma minority adolescents and their parents . Journal of Adolescence , 45 , 296–306. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.10.014 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dumas, T. , Ellis, W. , & Wolfe, D. (2012). Identity development as a buffer of adolescent risk behaviors in the context of peer group pressure and control . Journal of Adolescence , 35 , 917–927. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.12.012 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dunlop, W. L. (2015). Contextualized personality, beyond traits . European Journal of Personality , 29 , 310–325. 10.1002/per.1995 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dunlop, W. L. , Harake, N. , Gray, J. S. , Hanley, G. E. , & McCoy, T. P. (2018). The rises and falls of romance: Considering redemption, contamination, and affective tone in the narrative construction of love lives . Journal of Research in Personality , 74 , 23–29. 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.01.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Else‐Quest, N. M. , & Morse, E. (2015). Ethnic variations in parental ethnic socialization and adolescent ethnic identity: A longitudinal study . Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology , 21 , 54–64. 10.1037/a0037820 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erentaitė, R. , Vosylis, R. , Gabrialavičiūtė, I. , & Raižienė, S. (2018). How does school experience relate to adolescent identity formation over time? Cross‐lagged associations between school engagement, school burnout and identity processing styles . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 47 ( 4 ), 760–774. 10.1007/s10964-017-0806-1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis . New York: Norton. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eriksson, P. L. , McLean, K. C. , & Ann Frisén, A. (2020). Ta Det Onda Med Det Goda (Accepting the bad that comes with the good) – A cultural framework for identity narratives of difficult experiences in Sweden . Identity , 20 ( 3 ), 157–169. 10.1080/15283488.2020.1781636 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fivush, R. , Reese, E. , & Haden, C. A. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development . Child Development , 77 , 1568–1588. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Galliher, R. V. , McLean, K. C. , & Syed, M. (2017). An integrated developmental model for studying identity content in context . Developmental Psychology , 53 , 2011–2022. 10.1037/dev0000299 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grotevant, H. D. (1987). Toward a process model of identity formation . Journal of Adolescent Research , 2 , 203–222. 10.1177/074355488723003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grotevant, H. D. , & Cooper, C. R. (1985). Patterns of interaction in family relationships and the development of identity exploration in adolescence . Child Development , 56 , 415–428. 10.2307/1129730 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Habermas, T. , & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence of the life story in adolescence . Psychological Bulletin , 126 , 748. 10.1037/0033-2909.126.5.748 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Habermas, T. , & Köber, C. (2015). Autobiographical reasoning is constitutive for narrative identity: The role of the life story for person continuity. In McLean K. C., & Syed M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of identity development (pp. 149–165). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hatano, K. , Sugimura, K. , & Schwartz, S. J. (2018). Longitudinal links between identity consolidation and psychosocial problems in adolescence: Using bi‐factor latent change and cross‐lagged effect models . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 47 , 717–730. 10.1007/s10964-017-0785-2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hihara, S. , Sugimura, K. , & Syed, M. (2018). Forming a negative identity in contemporary society: Shedding light on the most problematic identity resolution . Identity , 18 ( 4 ), 325–333. 10.1080/15283488.2018.1524329 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hirschi, A. (2012). The career resources model: an integrative framework for career counsellors . British Journal of Guidance & Counselling , 40 , 369–383. 10.1080/03069885.2012.700506 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holm, T. , Thomsen, D. K. , & Bliksted, V. (2018). Themes of unfulfilled agency and communion in life stories of patients with schizophrenia . Psychiatry Research , 269 , 772–778. 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.116 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hu, A. W. , Zhou, X. , & Lee, R. M. (2017). Ethnic socialization and ethnic identity development among internationally adopted Korean American adolescents: A seven‐year follow‐up . Developmental Psychology , 53 , 2066–2077. 10.1037/dev0000421 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaniušonytė, G. , Truskauskaitė‐Kunevičienė, I. , Žukauskienė, R. , & Crocetti, E. (2019). Knowing who you are for not feeling lonely? A longitudinal study on identity and loneliness . Child Development , 90 , 1579–1588. 10.1111/cdev.13294 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kerpelman, J. L. , Pittman, J. F. , & Lamke, L. K. (1997). Toward a microprocess perspective on adolescent identity development: An identity control theory approach . Journal of Adolescent Research , 12 , 325–346. 10.1177/0743554897123002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kerpelman, J. , Pittman, J. , Saint‐Eloi Cadely, H. , Tuggle, F. , Harrell‐Levy, M. , & Adler‐Baeder, F. (2012). Identity and intimacy during adolescence: Connections among identity styles, romantic attachment and identity commitment . Journal of Adolescence , 35 , 1427–1439. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.03.008 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kiang, L. , Witkow, M. R. , Baldelomar, O. A. , & Fuligni, A. J. (2010). Change in ethnic identity across the high school years among adolescents with Latin American, Asian, and European backgrounds . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 39 , 683–693. 10.1007/s10964-009-9429-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kiang, L. , Witkow, M. R. , & Champagne, M. C. (2013). Normative changes in ethnic and American identities and links with adjustment among Asian American adolescents . Developmental Psychology , 49 , 1713–1722. 10.1037/a0030840 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klimstra, T. A. , & Denissen, J. (2017). A theoretical framework for the associations between identity and psychopathology . Developmental Psychology , 53 , 2052–2065. 10.1037/dev0000356 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klimstra, T. A. , Hale, W. W., III , Raaijmakers, Q. A. W. , Branje, S. J. T. , & Meeus, W. H. J. (2010). Identity formation in adolescence: Change or stability? Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 39 , 150–162. 10.1007/s10964-009-9401-4 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klimstra, T. A. , Kuppens, P. , Luyckx, K. , Branje, S. , Hale, W. W., 3rd , Oosterwegel, A. , … Meeus, W. (2016). Daily dynamics of adolescent mood and identity . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 26 , 459–473. 10.1111/jora.12205 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klimstra, T. A. , & Schwab, J. R. (2021). Time and identity: An evaluation of existing empirical research, conceptual challenges, and methodological opportunities . Identity , 1–14. 10.1080/15283488.2021.1924722 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koepke, S. , & Denissen, J. J. A. (2012). Dynamics of identity development and separation–individuation in parent–child relationships during adolescence and emerging adulthood – A conceptual integration . Developmental Review , 32 , 67–88. 10.1016/j.dr.2012.01.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Korobov, N. (2015). Identities as an interactional process. In McLean K. C., & Syed M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of identity development (pp. 210–227). New York: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kroger, J. (1996). Identity, regression and development . Journal of Adolescence , 19 , 203–222. 10.1006/jado.1996.0020 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kroger, J. , Martinussen, M. , & Marcia, J. E. (2010). Identity status change during adolescence and young adulthood: A meta‐analysis . Journal of Adolescence , 33 , 683–698. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.11.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kunnen, E. S. (2006). Are conflicts the motor in identity change? Identity , 6 , 169–186. 10.1207/s1532706xid0602_3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kunnen, E. S. (2010). Congruency between domains in commitment development . Identity , 10 , 254–269. 10.1080/15283488.2010.523321 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kunnen, E. S. (2021). The relation between vocational commitment and need fulfillment in real time experiences in clinical internships . Identity , 1–11. 10.1080/15283488.2021.1932899 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kunnen, E. S. , & Bosma, H. A. (2000). Development of meaning making: A dynamic systems approach . New Ideas in Psychology , 18 , 57–82. 10.1016/S0732-118X(99)00037-9 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kunnen, E. S. , Bosma, H. A. , van Halen, C. , & van der Meulen, M. (2001). A dynamic systems approach to identity and emotions: An overview and implications. In Bosma H. A., & Kunnen E. S. (Eds.), Identity and emotion: Development through self‐organization. (pp. 202–230). New York: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511598425.017 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levey, E. , Garandeau, C. F. , Meeus, W. , & Branje, S. (2019). The longitudinal role of self‐concept clarity and best friend delinquency in adolescent delinquent behavior . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 48 , 1068–1081. 10.1007/s10964-019-00997-1 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems for an integrated account of human development . Child Development , 71 , 36–43. https://doi‐org/10.1111/1467‐8624.00116 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lichtwarck‐Aschoff, A. , van Geert, P. , Bosma, H. , & Kunnen, S. (2008). Time and identity: A framework for research and theory formation . Developmental Review , 28 , 370–400. 10.1016/j.dr.2008.04.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lilgendahl, J. P. , & McLean, K. C. (2020). Narrative identity processes and patterns of adjustment across the transition to college: A developmentally contextualized approach . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 119 , 960–977. 10.1037/pspp0000277 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luyckx, K. , Goossens, L. , & Soenens, B. (2006). A developmental contextual perspective on identity construction in emerging adulthood: Change dynamics in commitment formation and commitment evaluation . Developmental Psychology , 42 , 366–380. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.366 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luyckx, K. , Klimstra, T. A. , Duriez, B. , Van Petegem, S. , Beyers, W. , Teppers, E. , & Goossens, L. (2013). Personal identity processes and self‐esteem: Temporal sequences in high school and college students . Journal of Research in Personality , 47 , 159–170. 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.10.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luyckx, K. , Seiffge‐Krenke, I. , Schwartz, S. J. , Crocetti, E. , & Klimstra, T. A. (2014). Identity configurations across love and work in emerging adults in romantic relationships . Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology , 35 , 192–203. 10.1016/j.appdev.2014.03.007 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego‐identity status . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 3 , 551–558. 10.1037/h0023281 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories . Review of General Psychology , 5 , 100–122. 10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.100 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McAdams, D. P. (2006). The redemptive self: Stories Americans live by . New York: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McAdams, D. P. , & McLean, K. C. (2013). Narrative identity . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 22 , 233–238. 10.1177/0963721413475622 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McAdams, D. P. , Reynolds, J. , Lewis, M. , Patten, A. H. , & Bowman, P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students . Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 27 , 474–485. 10.1177/0146167201274008 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , Breen, A. V. , & Fournier, A. C. (2010). Constructing the self in early, middle, and late adolescent boys: Narrative identity, individuation, and well‐being . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 20 , 166–187. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00633.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , & Jennings, L. E. (2012). Teens telling tales: How maternal and peer audiences support narrative identity development . Journal of Adolescence , 35 , 1455–1469. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.12.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , & Mansfield, C. D. (2011). To reason or not to reason: Is autobiographical reasoning always beneficial? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 131 , 85–97. 10.1002/cd.291 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , & Pasupathi, M. (2011). Old, new, borrowed, blue? The emergence and retention of personal meaning in autobiographical storytelling . Journal of Personality , 79 , 135–164. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00676.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , & Pasupathi, M. (2012). Processes of identity development: Where I am and how I got there . Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research , 12 , 8–28. 10.1080/15283488.2011.632363 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , Shucard, H. , & Syed, M. (2017). Applying the master narrative framework to gender identity development in emerging adulthood . Emerging Adulthood , 5 , 93–105. 10.1177/2167696816656254 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , & Syed, M. (2016). Personal, master, and alternative narratives: An integrative framework for understanding identity development in context . Human Development , 58 , 318–349. 10.1159/000445817 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , Syed, M. , Pasupathi, M. , Adler, J. M. , Dunlop, W. L. , Drustrup, D. , … McCoy, T. P. (2020). The empirical structure of narrative identity: The initial Big Three . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 119 , 920–944. 10.1037/pspp0000247 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , Syed, M. , & Shucard, H. (2016). Bringing identity content to the fore: Links to identity development processes . Emerging Adulthood , 4 , 356–364. 10.1177/2167696815626820 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLean, K. C. , Syed, M. , Yoder, A. , & Greenhoot, A. F. (2016). The role of domain content in understanding identity development processes . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 26 , 60–75. 10.1111/jora.12169 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meeus, W. (2011). The study of adolescent identity formation 2000–2010: A review of longitudinal research . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 21 , 75–94. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00716.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meeus, W. (2015). Why do young people become Jihadists? A theoretical account on radical identity development . European Journal of Developmental Psychology , 12 ( 3 ), 275–281. 10.1080/17405629.2015.1024106 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meeus, W. , van de Schoot, R. , Keijsers, L. , & Branje, S. (2012). Identity statuses as developmental trajectories: A five‐wave longitudinal study in early‐to‐middle and middle‐to‐late adolescents . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 41 , 1008–1021. 10.1007/s10964-011-9730-y [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meeus, W. , Van De Schoot, R. , Keijsers, L. , Schwartz, S. J. , & Branje, S. (2010). On the progression and stability of adolescent identity formation: A five‐wave longitudinal study in early‐to‐middle and middle‐to‐late adolescence . Child Development , 81 , 1565–1581. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01492.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mercer, N. , Crocetti, E. , Branje, S. , van Lier, P. , & Meeus, W. (2017). Linking delinquency and personal identity formation across adolescence: Examining between‐ and within‐person associations . Developmental Psychology , 53 , 2182–2194. 10.1037/dev0000351 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever . Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives , 2 , 201–218. 10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morsunbul, Ü. (2015). The effect of identity development, self‐esteem, low self‐control and gender on aggression in adolescence and emerging adulthood . Eurasian Journal of Educational Research , 61 , 99–116. 10.14689/ejer.2015.61.6 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moskowitz, D. S. , & Zuroff, D. C. (2004). Flux, pulse, and spin: Dynamic additions to the personality lexicon . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 86 , 880–893. 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.880 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Negru‐Subtirica, O. , Pop, E. I. , & Crocetti, E. (2015). Developmental trajectories and reciprocal associations between career adaptability and vocational identity: A three‐wave longitudinal study with adolescents . Journal of Vocational Behavior , 88 , 131–142. 10.1016/j.jvb.2015.03.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Negru‐Subtirica, O. , Pop, E. I. , Luyckx, K. , Dezutter, J. , & Steger, M. F. (2016). The meaningful identity: A longitudinal look at the interplay between identity and meaning in life in adolescence . Developmental Psychology , 52 , 1926–1936. 10.1037/dev0000176 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pasupathi, M. , & Hoyt, T. (2009). The development of narrative identity in late adolescence and emergent adulthood: The continued importance of listeners . Developmental Psychology , 45 , 558–574. 10.1037/a0014431 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pasupathi, M. , & Mansour, E. (2006). Adult age differences in autobiographical reasoningin narratives . Developmental Psychology , 42 , 798–808. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.798 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pasupathi, M. , Mansour, E. , & Brubaker, J. R. (2007). Developing a life story: Constructing relations between self and experience in autobiographical narratives . Human Development , 50 , 85–110. 10.1159/000100939 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Penner, F. , Gambin, M. , & Sharp, C. (2019). Childhood maltreatment and identity diffusion among inpatient adolescents: The role of reflective function . Journal of Adolescence , 76 , 65–74. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.08.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perez, T. , Cromley, J. G. , & Kaplan, A. (2014). The role of identity development, values, and costs in college STEM retention . Journal of Educational Psychology , 106 , 315–329. 10.1037/a0034027 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pittman, J. E. , Kerpelman, J. B. , Soto, F. M. , & Adler‐Baeder, M. (2012). Identity exploration in the dating domain: The role of attachment dimensions and parenting practices . Journal of Adolescence , 35 , 1485–1499. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.04.006 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pop, E. , Negru‐Subtirica, O. , Crocetti, E. , Opre, A. , & Meeus, W. (2016). On the interplay between academic achievement and educational identity: A longitudinal study . Journal of Adolescence , 47 , 135–144. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.11.004 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ragelienė, T. (2016). Links of adolescents identity development and relationship with peers: A systematic literature review . Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry = Journal De L'academie Canadienne De Psychiatrie De L'enfant Et De L'adolescent , 25 , 97–105. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reese, E. , Haden, C. A. , Baker‐Ward, L. , Bauer, P. , Fivush, R. , & Ornstein, P. A. (2011). Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: A multidimensional model and coding method . Journal of Cognition and Development , 12 , 424–462. 10.1080/15248372.2011.587854 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rich, Y. , & Schachter, E. (2012). High school identity climate and student identity development . Contemporary Educational Psychology , 37 , 218–228. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.06.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rogers, L. O. , Scott, M. A. , & Way, N. (2015). Racial and gender identity among Black adolescent males: An intersectionality perspective . Child Development , 86 , 407–424. 10.1111/cdev.12303 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schachter, E. P. (2018). Intergenerational, unconscious, and embodied: Three underdeveloped aspects of Erikson’s theory of identity . Identity , 18 ( 4 ), 315–324. 10.1080/15283488.2018.1523731 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schoon, I. (2015). Diverse pathways: Rethinking the transition to adulthood. In Amato P. R., Booth A., McHale S., & Van Hook J. (Eds.), Families in an era of increasing inequality. Diverging destinies (pp. 115–136). London, UK: Springer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz, S. J. , Côté, J. E. , & Arnett, J. J. (2005). Identity and agency in emerging adulthood: Two developmental routes in the individualization process . Youth & Society , 37 , 201–229. 10.1177/0044118X05275965 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz, S. J. , Klimstra, T. A. , Luyckx, K. , Hale, W. W., III , Frijns, T. , Oosterwegel, A. , … Meeus, W. H. J. (2011). Daily dynamics of personal identity and self‐concept clarity . European Journal of Personality , 25 , 373–385. 10.1002/per.798 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz, S. J. , Zamboanga, B. L. , & Weisskirch, R. S. (2008). Broadening the study of the self: Integrating the study of personal identity and cultural identity . Social and Personality Psychology Compass , 2 , 635–651. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00077.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Selfhout, M. H. W. , Branje, S. J. T. , & Meeus, W. H. J. (2009). Developmental trajectories of perceived friendship intimacy, conflict resolution, and depression from early to late adolescence . Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 37 , 251–264. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shirai, T. , Nakamura, T. , & Katsuma, K. (2016). Identity development in relation to time beliefs in emerging adulthood: A long‐term longitudinal study . Identity , 16 , 45–58. 10.1080/15283488.2015.1121817 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Slotter, E. B. , & Gardner, W. L. (2011). Can you help me become the “me” I want to be? The role of goal pursuit in friendship formation . Self and Identity , 10 , 231–247. 10.1080/15298868.2010.482767 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sugimura, K. , Crocetti, E. , Hatano, K. , Kaniušonytė, G. , Hihara, S. , & Žukauskienė, R. (2018). A cross‐cultural perspective on the relationships between emotional separation, parental trust, and identity in adolescents . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 47 , 749–759. 10.1007/s10964-018-0819-4 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sugimura, K. , Gmelin, J. O. H. , van der Gaag, M. A. , & Kunnen, E. S. (2021). Exploring exploration: Identity exploration in real‐time interactions among peers . Identity , 1–18. 10.1080/15283488.2021.1947819 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swann, W. B. Jr. (2000). Identity negotiation: where two roads meet , & (Eds.), Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . 53 , (pp. 1038–1051). 10.1037//0022-3514.53.6.1038 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swann, W. B. , Milton, L. P. , & Polzer, J. T. (2000). Should we create a niche or fall in line? Negotiation and small group effectiveness . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 79 , 238–250. 10.1037//0022-3514.79.2.238 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Syed, M. , & Azmitia, M. (2010). Narrative and ethnic identity exploration: A longitudinal account of emerging adults’ ethnicity‐related experiences . Developmental Psychology , 46 , 208–219. 10.1037/a0017825 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Syed, M. , Juang, L. P. , & Svensson, Y. (2018). Toward a new understanding of ethnic‐racial settings for ethnic‐racial identity development . Journal of Research on Adolescence , 28 ( 2 ), 262–276. 10.1111/jora.12387 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Umaña‐Taylor, A. J. , Douglass, S. , Updegraff, K. A. , & Marsiglia, F. F. (2018). A small‐scale randomized efficacy trial of the Identity Project: Promoting adolescents’ ethnic–racial identity exploration and resolution . Child Development , 89 ( 3 ), 862–870. https://doi‐org/10.1111/cdev.12755 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Umaña‐Taylor, A. J. , Kornienko, O. , Bayless, S. D. , & Updegraff, K. A. (2018). A universal intervention program increases ethnic‐racial identity exploration and resolution to predict adolescent psychosocial functioning one year later . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 47 ( 1 ), 1–15. 10.1007/s10964-017-0766-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Umaña‐Taylor, A. J. , Quintana, S. M. , Lee, R. M. , Cross, W. E. , Rivas‐Drake, D. , Schwartz, S. J. , … Ethnic and Racial Identity in the 21st Century Study Group (2014). Ethnic and racial identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An integrated conceptualization . Child Development , 85 ( 1 ), 21–39. 10.1111/cdev.12196 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van der Gaag, M. A. E. , Albers, C. J. , & Kunnen, E. S. (2017). Micro‐level mechanisms of identity development: The role of emotional experiences in commitment development . Developmental Psychology , 53 , 2205–2217. 10.1037/dev0000336 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van der Gaag, M. A. , de Ruiter, N. M. , & Kunnen, E. S. (2016). Micro‐level processes of identity development: Intra‐individual relations between commitment and exploration . Journal of Adolescence , 47 , 38–47. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.11.007 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Doeselaar, C. , Meeus, W. , Koot, H. M. , & Branje, S. (2016). The role of best friends in educational identity formation in adolescence . Journal of Adolescence , 47 , 28–37. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.12.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Doeselaar, L. , Becht, A. I. , Klimstra, T. A. , & Meeus, W. H. J. (2018). A review and integration of three key components of identity development: Distinctiveness, coherence, and continuity . European Psychologist , 23 , 278–288. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000334 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Doeselaar, L. , Klimstra, T. A. , Denissen, J. J. A. , Branje, S. , & Meeus, W. H. J. (2018). The role of identity commitments in depressive symptoms and stressful life events in adolescence and young adulthood . Developmental Psychology , 54 , 950–962. 10.1037/dev0000479 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Doeselaar, L. , McLean, K. C. , Meeus, W. , Denissen, J. J. A. , & Klimstra, T. A. (2020). Adolescents’ identity formation: Linking the narrative and the dual‐cycle approach . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 49 , 818–835. 10.1007/s10964-019-01096-x [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vosylis, R. , Erentaitė, R. , & Crocetti, E. (2018). Global versus domain‐specific identity processes: Which domains are more relevant for emerging adults? Emerging Adulthood , 6 , 32–41. 10.1177/2167696817694698 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang, Y. , Douglass, S. , & Yip, T. (2017). Longitudinal relations between ethnic/racial identity process and content: Exploration, commitment, and salience among diverse adolescents . Developmental Psychology , 53 ( 11 ), 2154–2169. 10.1037/dev0000388 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wantchekon, K. A. , & Umaña‐Taylor, A. J. (2021). Relating profiles of ethnic‐racial identity process and content to the academic and psychological adjustment of Black and Latinx adolescents . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 50 , 1333–1352. https://doi‐org/10.1007/s10964‐021‐01451‐x [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waterman, A. S. (1982). Identity development from adolescence to adulthood: An extension of theory and a review of research . Developmental Psychology , 18 , 341–358. 10.1037/0012-1649.18.3.341 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waterman, A. S. (2015). What does it mean to engage in identity exploration and to hold identity commitments? A methodological critique of multidimensional measures for the study of identity processes . Identity , 15 , 309–349. 10.1080/15283488.2015.1089403 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waterman, A. S. (2020). “Now what do I do?“: Toward a conceptual understanding of the effects of traumatic events on identity functioning . Journal of Adolescence , 79 , 59–69. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.11.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waters, T. E. , & Fivush, R. (2015). Relations between narrative coherence, identity, and psychological well‐being in emerging adulthood . Journal of Personality , 83 , 441–451. 10.1111/jopy.12120 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waters, T. E. A. , Shallcross, J. F. , & Fivush, R. (2013). The many facets of meaning making: Comparing multiple measures of meaning making and their relations to psychological distress . Memory , 21 , 111–124. 10.1080/09658211.2012.705300 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wiley, R. , & Berman, S. (2012). The relationships among caregiver and adolescent identity status, identity distress and psychological adjustment . Journal of Adolescence , 35 , 1203–1213. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.04.001 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wu, J. , Watkins, D. , & Hattie, J. (2010). Self‐concept clarity: A longitudinal study of Hong Kong adolescents . Personality and Individual Differences , 48 , 277–282. 10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.011 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yip, T. , Wang, Y. , Mootoo, C. , & Mirpuri, S. (2019). Moderating the association between discrimination and adjustment: A meta‐analysis of ethnic/racial identity . Developmental Psychology , 55 , 1274–1298. 10.1037/dev0000708 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Identity Theory

    identity theory research paper

  2. Erik Erikson's Identity Theory

    identity theory research paper

  3. Identity Essay

    identity theory research paper

  4. (PDF) The Development of Identity Theory

    identity theory research paper

  5. Erik Erikson's Identity Theory

    identity theory research paper

  6. Self identity paper. Self Identity Research Papers. 2019-02-11

    identity theory research paper

VIDEO

  1. Knots and Links in Spatial Graphs

  2. social identity theory { Ugc Net Paper 2} Social Psychology

  3. Yiddish, an Indelible Part of My Identity

  4. identity theory EP song

  5. Imogene King: Goal Attainment Theory

  6. Decoding Social Identity Theory

COMMENTS

  1. Advancing Identity Theory: Examining the Relationship between Activated

    Over the past decades identity theory has emerged as a robust research program in sociological social psychology (Burke and Stets 2009). Identity theorists have attempted to explain how identities operate within the self. Within identity theory there are different perspectives—or emphases—that describe how identities operate to motivate ...

  2. Full article: The Role of Identity in Human Behavior Research: A

    This review investigated the identity theory alignment of existing behavior research by systematically searching, screening, and reviewing identity-behavior relevant papers, across three behavioral psychology databases. ... The first phase was based on article title and required the title to be suggestive of 1) an original (applied) research ...

  3. (PDF) Identity Theory

    An Identity. An identity is a set of meanings attached to roles individuals occupy in the social structure (Stryker, [1980] 2002 ) (role identities), groups they identify with and belong to (group ...

  4. The Past, Present, and Future of an Identity Theory

    identity theory; in the rest of this paper we retain that usage to simplify presentation. Identity theory has evolved in two some-what different but closely related directions. Both are instantiations of a theoretical and research program labeled structural symbolic interactionism (Stryker 1980), whose goal is to understand and explain how ...

  5. (PDF) Self-Identity Theory and Research Methods

    San Francisco, CA 94143-0984, UNITED STATES. [email protected]. Abstract. This article introduces the focus and methods of clinical psychodynamic research for. researchers and professionals ...

  6. Identities: A developmental social-psychological perspective

    From Erikson's psychosocial theory to identity process-oriented models. Erikson's (Citation 1950, Citation 1968) psychosocial theory has inspired research on personal identity up to the present time (Schachter & Galliher, Citation 2018).A fundamental tenet of Erikson's view is that identity formation is a core task that individuals strive to address throughout their entire life span ...

  7. Identity

    Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, under the sponsorship of the International Society for Research on Identity (ISRI), provides an interdisciplinary and international publication outlet for conceptual, empirical, and methodological developments and emerging trends in the field of identity research.. The Journal brings together leading research in identity undertaken by ...

  8. Handbook of Identity Theory and Research

    Seth J. Schwartz, Koen Luyckx, Vivian L. Vignoles. Is the only handbook in which the different streams of identity theory and research are brought together in one concise resource. Offers 40 chapters written by recognized experts in their respective fields, examining the most cutting-edge identity theory, research, and applications available.

  9. The Identity Statuses: Origins, Meanings, and Interpretations

    The identity statuses—on which much current identity theory and research is based (Kroger, 2007 )—originated from attempts to validate a major construct, ego identity, drawn from Erikson's (1950) ego psychoanalytic theory. In this chapter, James Marcia begins by detailing the origins and meanings of the identity statuses.

  10. Identity Theory and Social Identity Theory

    In social identity theory, a social identity is a person's knowledge that he or she. belongs to a social category or group (Hogg and Abrams 1988). A social group is a set of. individuals who hold a common social identi- fication or view themselves as members of the same social category.

  11. Dynamics of Identity Development in Adolescence: A Decade in Review

    In the current review, we discuss progress in the field of identity research between 2010 and 2020. One of the key developmental tasks in adolescence and young adulthood is to develop a coherent sense of self and identity (Erikson, 1968).Personal identity refers to one's sense of the person one genuinely is, including a subjective feeling of self-sameness and continuity over contexts and time.

  12. Social Identity Theory

    Based on the aforementioned hypotheses, three theoretical principles are formed: (1) Individuals strive to achieve or maintain positive social identity. (2) Positive social identity is developed on the basis of favorable comparison between the ingroup and the relevant outgroup. The ingroup perception must be positive and distinct from that of ...

  13. Social Identity Theory

    Abstract. Social identity theory (SIT) provides a framework for explaining intergroup behavior and intergroup communication based on the inherent value humans place on social group memberships, and their desire to view their specific social groups in a positive light. This desire can lead to intergroup prejudice and conflict.

  14. Identity Development in Adolescence and Adulthood

    The means by which one experiences a feeling of sameness in the midst of continual change is the focus of identity theory and research. Historically, concerns with questions of identity are relatively recent. ... Subsequent to the original Identity Status Interview, several paper-and-pencil measures were developed to assess Marcia's four ...

  15. (PDF) Self, self-concept, and identity

    take in the world -- is a core se lf -project. Self and identity theories assume that people care. about themselves, want to know who they are, and can use this self-knowledge to make sense of ...

  16. Social status and the pursuit of positive social identity: Systematic

    Social identity theory and intergroup discrimination. Since the minimal group studies (Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971), research in SIT has focused on the cognitive and motivational processes underlying intergroup discrimination.SIT argues that discrimination is driven by a fundamental motivation to maintain a positive and distinctive social identity (Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986).

  17. The Landscape of Identity Model: An Integration of Qualitative and

    of the field of identity research, and indeed for our understanding of what identity truly is. ... In this paper, we aim to remedy some of the vagueness surrounding the identity concept by ... identity theory, proposed a fundamental qualitative aspect of identity commitments: the content of the domain of life in which the commitment can be ...

  18. The Development of Self and Identity in Adolescence

    In summary, in research using functional neuroimaging, explicit self-evaluation as well as more indirect forms of social self-evaluation implicated in relational identity robustly engage the vmPFC and the rostral/perigenual ACC (as part of a broader network including the VS, TPJ, and dmPFC) in children and adolescents, often more so than in adults.

  19. (PDF) Social Identity Theory

    Introduction. Social Identity Theory (SIT; Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel. & Turner, 1979) begins with the premise that. individuals define their own identities with regard. to social groups and that such ...

  20. Dynamics of Identity Development in Adolescence: A Decade in Review

    In the current review, we discuss progress in the field of identity research between 2010 and 2020. One of the key developmental tasks in adolescence and young adulthood is to develop a coherent sense of self and identity (Erikson, 1968).Personal identity refers to one's sense of the person one genuinely is, including a subjective feeling of self‐sameness and continuity over contexts and time.

  21. What perspectives underlie 'researcher identity'? A review of two

    Over the past two decades, identity has emerged as a concept framing studies of early career researcher experience. Yet, identity is an amorphous concept, understood and used in a range of ways. This systematic review aimed to unpack the underpinnings of the notion of researcher identity. The final sample consisted of 38 empirical articles published in peer-reviewed journals in the last 20 ...

  22. (PDF) Social Identity Theory

    Abstract. Social identity theory (SIT) provides a framework for explaining intergroup behavior and intergroup communication based on the inherent value humans place on social group memberships ...

  23. Understanding Knowledge Sharing From an Identity-Based Motivational

    Research typically adopted a social exchange perspective to suggest that employees share their knowledge with coworkers to reciprocate prior positive treatment to return the favor. We challenge this dominant focus on external motivational sources and adopt an identity-based motivational perspective. Our theorizing is grounded in identity theory and recognizes knowledge-sharing identity ...

  24. Social Identity Theory

    Social Identity Theory (SIT; Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1979) begins with the premise that individuals define their own identities with regard to social groups and that such identifications work to protect and bolster self-identity.The creation of group identities involves both the categorization of one's "in-group" with regard to an "out-group" and the tendency to view one's ...

  25. Fifty Years Since "Identity: Youth and Crisis": A Renewed Look at

    This special issue of Identity was inspired by the semicentennial of Erikson's landmark book "Identity: Youth and Crisis," published in Citation 1968.Erikson wrote on his concept of identity both before and after 1968. This particular book, mostly comprising previously published essays, was not compiled in order to present a systematically organized exposition of the concept of identity ...